Old page wikitext, before the edit (old_wikitext ) | '<!-- This page is highly dated and has a lot of archaic jargon; please clean it! -->
{{Infobox brain
| Name = Visual cortex
| Latin = Cortex visualis
| GraySubject =
| GrayPage =
| Image = Brodmann areas 17 18 19.png
| Caption = View of the brain from behind. Red = Brodmann area 17 (primary visual cortex); orange = area 18; yellow = area 19
| Image2 = Brodmann Cytoarchitectonics 17.png
| Caption2 = Brain shown from the side, facing left. Above: view from outside, below: cut through the middle. Orange = Brodmann area 17 (primary visual cortex)
| IsPartOf =
| Components =
| Artery =
| Vein =
| BrainInfoType =
| BrainInfoNumber =
| MeshName =
| MeshNumber =
| DorlandsPre = c_57
| DorlandsSuf = 12261838
|NeuroLex = Visual cortex primary
|NeuroLexID = nlx_143552
}}The '''visual cortex''' of the [[brain]] is a part of the [[cerebral cortex]] that processes [[visual perception|visual information]]. It is located in the [[occipital lobe]] in the back of the head.
Visual information coming from the eye goes through the [[lateral geniculate nucleus]] in the [[thalamus]] and then reaches the visual cortex. The part of the visual cortex that receives the sensory inputs from the [[thalamus]] is the primary visual cortex, also known as visual area 1 (V1), and the striate cortex. The extrastriate areas consist of visual areas 2 (V2), 3 (V3), 4 (V4), and 5 (V5).<ref>{{cite web|last1=Mather|first1=George|title=The Visual Cortex|url=http://www.lifesci.sussex.ac.uk/home/George_Mather/Linked%20Pages/Physiol/Cortex.html|website=School of Life Sciences: University of Sussex|publisher=University of Sussex|accessdate=6 March 2017|language=English}}</ref>
Both [[cerebral hemisphere|hemispheres of the brain]] contain a visual cortex; the visual cortex in the left hemisphere receives signals from the right [[visual field]], and the visual cortex in the right hemisphere receives signals from the left visual field.
==Introduction==
The primary visual cortex (V1) is located in and around the [[calcarine fissure]] in the [[occipital lobe]]. Each hemisphere's V1 receives information directly from its ipsilateral [[lateral geniculate nucleus]] that receives signals from the contralateral visual hemifield.
Neurons in the visual cortex fire [[action potential]]s when visual stimuli appear within their [[receptive field]]. By definition, the receptive field is the region within the entire visual field that elicits an [[action potential]]. But, for any given neuron, it may respond best to a subset of stimuli within its receptive field. This property is called ''[[neuronal tuning]]''. In the earlier visual areas, neurons have simpler tuning. For example, a neuron in V1 may fire to any vertical stimulus in its receptive field. In the higher visual areas, neurons have complex tuning. For example, in the inferior temporal cortex (IT), a neuron may fire only when a certain face appears in its receptive field.
The visual cortex receives its blood supply primarily from the [[calcarine artery|calcarine branch]] of the [[posterior cerebral artery]].
[[File:Neural pathway diagram.svg|thumb]]
== Psychological model of the neural processing of visual information ==
{{Main article|Two-streams hypothesis}}
=== Ventral-dorsal model ===
[[File:Ventral-dorsal streams.svg|thumb|right|300px|The [[dorsal stream]] (green) and [[ventral stream]] (purple) are shown. They originate from primary visual cortex.]]
Each V1 transmits information to two primary pathways, called the ventral stream and the dorsal stream.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
* The '''[[Two Streams hypothesis#Ventral stream|ventral stream]]''' begins with V1, goes through visual area V2, then through visual area V4, and to the [[inferior temporal cortex]] (IT cortex). The ventral stream, sometimes called the "What Pathway", is associated with form recognition and object representation. It is also associated with storage of [[long-term memory]].
* The '''[[Two Streams hypothesis#Dorsal stream|dorsal stream]]''' begins with V1, goes through Visual area V2, then to the [[dorsomedial area]] (DM/ V6) and Visual area MT (middle temporal/ V5) and to the [[posterior parietal cortex]]. The dorsal stream, sometimes called the "Where Pathway" or "How Pathway", is associated with motion, representation of object locations, and control of the eyes and arms, especially when visual information is used to guide [[saccade]]s or reaching.
The what vs. where account of the ventral/dorsal pathways was first described by [[Leslie Ungerleider|Ungerleider]] and [[Mortimer Mishkin|Mishkin]].<ref name="UngerleiderMishkin82">{{cite book|title=Analysis of Visual Behavior|vauthors=Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M|publisher=MIT Press|year=1982|veditors=Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJ|location=Boston|pages=549–586|chapter=Two Cortical Visual Systems}}</ref>
More recently, [[Melvyn A. Goodale|Goodale]] and Milner extended these ideas and suggested that the ventral stream is critical for visual perception whereas the dorsal stream mediates the visual control of skilled actions.<ref name="GoodaleMilner">{{cite journal|year=1992|title=Separate pathways for perception and action.|journal=Trends in Neurosciences|volume=15|issue=1|pages=20–25|doi=10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8|pmid=1374953|vauthors=Goodale MA, Milner AD}}</ref> It has been shown that visual illusions such as the [[Ebbinghaus illusion]] distort judgements of a perceptual nature, but when the subject responds with an action, such as grasping, no distortion occurs.<ref name="Aglioti1995">{{cite journal|year=1995|title=Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand.|journal=Curr. Biol.|volume=5|issue=6|pages=679–85|doi=10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3|pmid=7552179|vauthors=Aglioti S, DeSouza JF, Goodale MA}}</ref>
Work such as the one from Scharnowski and Gegenfurtner<ref name="Franz2005">{{cite journal|year=2005|title=Illusion effects on grasping are temporally constant not dynamic.|journal=J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform.|volume=31|issue=6|pages=1359–78|doi=10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1359|pmid=16366795|author=Franz VH, Scharnowski F, Gegenfurtner}}</ref> suggests that both the action and perception systems are equally fooled by such illusions. Other studies, however, provide strong support for the idea that skilled actions such as grasping are not affected by pictorial illusions<ref name="Ganel2003">{{cite journal|year=2003|title=Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape.|journal=Nature|volume=426|issue=6967|pages=664–7|doi=10.1038/nature02156|pmid=14668865|vauthors=Ganel T, Goodale MA}}</ref><ref name="Ganel2008">{{cite journal|year=2008|title=A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: opposite effects of real and illusory size.|journal=Psych. Sci.|volume=19|issue=3|pages=221–5|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02071.x|pmid=18315792|vauthors=Ganel T, Tanzer M, Goodale MA}}</ref> and suggest that the action/perception dissociation is a useful way to characterize the functional division of labor between the dorsal and ventral visual pathways in the cerebral cortex.<ref name="Goodale2011">{{cite journal|year=2011|title=Transforming vision into action.|journal=Vision Res.|volume=51|issue=14|pages=1567–87|doi=10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.027|pmid=20691202|author=Goodale MA.}}</ref>
== Primary visual cortex (V1)==
{{Refimprove section|date=November 2016}}
[[File:visualcortex.gif|right]]
[[File:Visual cortex - low mag.jpg|thumb|righ|[[Micrograph]] showing the visual cortex (pink). The [[pia mater]] and [[arachnoid mater]] including [[blood vessel]]s are seen at the top of the image. Subcortical [[white matter]] (blue) is seen at the bottom of the image. [[LFB stain|HE-LFB stain]].]]
The '''primary visual cortex''' is the most studied visual area in the [[brain]]. In mammals, it is located in the posterior pole of the occipital lobe and is the simplest, earliest cortical visual area. It is highly specialized for processing information about static and moving objects and is excellent in [[pattern recognition]].{{Clarify|reason=unclear what "highly specialized" and "excellent" means in this sentence.|date=November 2016}}
The functionally defined primary visual cortex is approximately equivalent to the anatomically defined '''striate cortex'''.{{Clarify|date=November 2016}} The name "striate cortex" is derived from the [[line of Gennari]], a distinctive stripe visible to the naked eye<ref>Glickstein M., Rizzolatti G. ''Francesco Gennari and the structure of the cerebral cortex'' Trends in Neurosciences, Volume 7, Issue 12, 464-467, 1 December 1984.</ref> that represents [[myelin]]ated [[axons]] from the [[lateral geniculate body]] terminating in layer 4 of the [[gray matter]].
The primary visual cortex is divided into six functionally distinct layers, labeled 1 to 6. Layer 4, which receives most visual input from the [[lateral geniculate nucleus]] (LGN), is further divided into 4 layers, labelled 4A, 4B, 4Cα, and 4Cβ. Sublamina 4Cα{{Clarify|reason=unclear what a sublamina is|date=November 2016}} receives mostly [[Magnocellular cell|magnocellular]] input from the LGN, while layer 4Cβ receives input from [[Parvocellular cell|parvocellular]] pathways.
The average number of neurons in the adult human primary visual cortex in each hemisphere has been estimated at around 140 million.<ref name="Leuba-Kraftsik-1994">{{cite journal |author1= Leuba G |author2= Kraftsik R |year= 1994 |title=Changes in volume, surface estimate, three-dimensional shape and total number of neurons of the human primary visual cortex from midgestation until old age |journal=Anatomy and Embryology |volume=190 |issue=4 |pages=351–366 |publisher= |doi= 10.1007/BF00187293|pmid=7840422 |pmc= |url= |accessdate=}}</ref>
=== Function ===
{{relevance inline|date=September 2016}}
{{technical|date=September 2016}}
V1 has a very well-defined map of the spatial information in vision. For example, in humans, the upper bank of the [[calcarine sulcus]] responds strongly to the lower half of [[visual field]] (below the center), and the lower bank of the calcarine to the upper half of visual field. In concept, this [[retinotopy|retinotopic]] mapping is a transformation of the visual image from [[retina]] to V1. The correspondence between a given location in V1 and in the subjective visual field is very precise: even the [[Blind spot (vision)|blind spots]] are mapped into V1. In terms of evolution, this correspondence is very basic and found in most animals that possess a V1. In humans and animals with a [[Fovea centralis|fovea]] in the retina, a large portion of V1 is mapped to the small, central portion of visual field, a phenomenon known as [[cortical magnification]].<ref>{{cite book|last1=Barghout|first1=Lauren|title=On the Differences Between Peripheral and Foveal Pattern Masking|date=1999|publisher=Masters Thesis. U.C. Berkeley|location=Berkeley, California, U.S.A.}}</ref> Perhaps for the purpose of accurate spatial encoding, neurons in V1 have the smallest [[receptive field]] size of any visual cortex microscopic regions.
The tuning properties of V1 neurons (what the neurons respond to) differ greatly over time. Early in time (40 ms and further) individual V1 neurons have strong tuning to a small set of stimuli. That is, the neuronal responses can discriminate small changes in visual [[Orientation (mental)|orientations]], [[spatial frequencies]] and [[color]]s. Furthermore, individual V1 neurons in human and animals with [[binocular vision]] have ocular dominance, namely tuning to one of the two eyes. In V1, and primary sensory cortex in general, neurons with similar tuning properties tend to cluster together as [[cortical column]]s. [[David Hubel]] and [[Torsten Wiesel]] proposed the classic ice-cube organization model of cortical columns for two tuning properties: [[ocular dominance columns|ocular dominance]] and orientation. However, this model cannot accommodate the color, spatial frequency and many other features to which neurons are tuned {{Citation needed|date=November 2011}}. The exact organization of all these cortical columns within V1 remains a hot topic of current research. The mathematical modeling of this function has been compared to [[Gabor transform]]s.
Later in time (after 100 ms), neurons in V1 are also sensitive to the more global organisation of the scene (Lamme & Roelfsema, 2000).<ref>{{cite book|last1=Barghout|first1=Lauren|title=Vision: How Global Perceptual Context Changes Local Contrast Processing (Ph.D. Dissertation). Updated to include computer vision techniques|date=2003|publisher=Scholar's Press|isbn=978-3-639-70962-9|url=https://www.morebooks.de/store/gb/book/vision/isbn/978-3-639-70962-9}}</ref> These response properties probably stem from recurrent [[feedback]] processing (the influence of higher-tier cortical areas on lower-tier cortical areas) and lateral connections from [[Pyramidal cell|pyramidal neurons]] (Hupe et al. 1998). While feedforward connections are mainly driving, feedback connections are mostly modulatory in their effects (Angelucci et al., 2003; Hupe et al., 2001). Evidence shows that feedback originating in higher-level areas such as V4, IT, or MT, with bigger and more complex receptive fields, can modify and shape V1 responses, accounting for contextual or [[extra-classical receptive field]] effects (Guo et al., 2007; Huang et al., 2007; Sillito et al., 2006).
The visual information relayed to V1 is not coded in terms of spatial (or optical) imagery but rather are better described as [[edge detection]]. As an example, for an image comprising half side black and half side white, the dividing line between black and white has strongest local contrast (that is, edge detection) and is encoded, while few neurons code the brightness information (black or white per se). As information is further relayed to subsequent visual areas, it is coded as increasingly non-local frequency/phase signals. Note that, at these early stages of cortical visual processing, spatial location of visual information is well preserved amid the local contrast encoding (edge detection).
Axiomatically{{clarify|date=September 2016}} determined functional models of simple cells in V1 have been determined by Lindeberg<ref name=Lin13BICY>{{cite journal | last1 = Lindeberg | first1 = T. | year = | title = A computational theory of visual receptive fields | doi = 10.1007/s00422-013-0569-z | journal = Biological Cybernetics | volume = 107 | issue = 6| pages = 589–635 }}</ref><ref name=Lin16-JMIV>{{cite journal | last1 = Lindeberg | first1 = T. | year = | title = Time-causal and time-recursive spatio-temporal receptive fields | url = http://www.csc.kth.se/~tony/abstracts/Lin16-JMIV.html | journal = Journal of Mathematical Imaging and Vision | volume = 55 | issue = 1| pages = 50–88 | doi=10.1007/s10851-015-0613-9}}</ref> in terms of directional derivatives{{clarify|date=September 2016}} of affine Gaussian kernels{{clarify|date=September 2016}} over the spatial domain{{clarify|date=September 2016}} in combination with temporal derivatives{{clarify|date=September 2016}} of either non-causal or time-causal scale-space kernels{{clarify|date=September 2016}} over the temporal domain (see [[axiomatic theory of receptive fields]]). Specifically, it has been shown that this theory both leads to predictions about receptive fields with good qualitative agreement with the biological receptive field measurements performed by DeAngelis et al.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = DeAngelis | first1 = G. C. | last2 = Ohzawa | first2 = I. | last3 = Freeman | first3 = R. D. | year = 1995 | title = Receptive field dynamics in the central visual pathways | url = | journal = Trends Neurosci | volume = 18 | issue = 10| pages = 451–457 | doi=10.1016/0166-2236(95)94496-r | pmid=8545912}}</ref><ref>G. C. DeAngelis and A. Anzai "A modern view of the classical receptive field: linear and non-linear spatio-temporal processing by V1 neurons. In: Chalupa, L.M., Werner, J.S. (eds.) The Visual Neurosciences, vol. 1, pp. 704–719. MIT Press, Cambridge, 2004.</ref> and guarantees good theoretical properties of the mathematical receptive field model, including covariance and invariance properties under natural image transformations.<ref name=Lin13PONE>{{cite journal | last1 = Lindeberg | first1 = T. | year = | title = Invariance of visual operations at the level of receptive fields | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0066990 | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 8 | issue = 7| page = e66990 }}</ref>{{relevance inline|date=September 2016}}
Differences in size of V1 also seem to have an effect on the [[Interindividual differences in perception|perception of illusions]].<ref name="surface_V1">{{cite journal | url=http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v14/n1/full/nn.2706.html | title=The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size | author=Schwarzkopf, D Samuel | journal=Nature Neuroscience | volume=14 | year=2011 | issue=1 | pages=28–30 | doi=10.1038/nn.2706 | pmid=21131954 | pmc=3012031}}</ref>
== V2 == <!--Please keep this header, as it is a redirect -->
'''Visual area V2''', or '''secondary visual cortex''', also called '''prestriate cortex''',<ref>Gazzaniga, Ivry & Mangun: ''Cognitive neuroscience,'' 2002</ref> is the second major area in the visual cortex, and the first region within the '''visual association area'''. It receives strong [[Feedforward neural network|feedforward]] connections from V1 (direct and via the pulvinar) and sends strong connections to V3, V4, and V5. It also sends strong [[feedback]] connections to V1.
In terms of anatomy, V2 is split into four quadrants, a [[Dorsum (biology)|dorsal]] and [[ventral]] representation in the left and the right [[cerebral hemisphere|hemispheres]]. Together, these four regions provide a complete map of the visual world. V2 has many properties in common with V1: Cells are tuned to simple properties such as orientation, spatial frequency, and colour. The responses of many V2 neurons are also modulated by more complex properties, such as the orientation of [[illusory contours]],<ref name="illusory contours">{{cite journal|last1=von der Heydt|first1=R|last2=Peterhans|first2=E|last3=Baumgartner|first3=G|title=Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses|journal=Science|date=1984|volume=224|pages=1260–62|doi=10.1126/science.6539501|pmid=6539501}}</ref><ref name="A. Anzai, X. Peng 2007"/> [[binocular disparity]],<ref name="stereoscopic edges">{{cite journal|last1=von der Heydt|first1=R|last2=Zhou|first2=H|last3=Friedman|first3=H. S|title=Representation of stereoscopic edges in monkey visual cortex|journal=Vision Research|date=2000|volume=40|pages=1955–67|doi=10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00044-4}}</ref> and whether the stimulus is part of the figure or the ground.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Qiu|first1=F. T|last2=von der Heydt|first2=R|title=Figure and ground in the visual cortex: V2 combines stereoscopic cues with Gestalt rules|journal=Neuron|date=2005|volume=47|pages=155–66|url=https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.028|doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.028}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Maruko, I; et alt.|title=Postnatal Development of Disparity Sensitivity in Visual Area 2 (V2) of Macaque Monkeys|journal=Journal of Neurophysiology|date=2008|volume=100|issue=5|pages=2486–2495|doi=10.1152/jn.90397.2008|pmid=18753321|pmc=2585398}}</ref> Recent research has shown that V2 cells show a small amount of attentional modulation (more than V1, less than V4), are tuned for moderately complex patterns, and may be driven by multiple orientations at different subregions within a single receptive field.
It is argued that the entire ventral visual-to-hippocampal stream is important for visual memory.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Bussey|first1=T J|last2=Saksida|first2=L. M|title=Memory, perception, and the ventral visual-perirhinal-hippocampal stream: thinking outside of the boxes|journal=Hippocampus|date=2007|volume=17|issue=9|pages = 898–908|doi=10.1002/hipo.20320|pmid=17636546}}</ref> This theory, unlike the dominant one, predicts that object-recognition memory (ORM) alterations could result from the manipulation in V2, an area that is highly interconnected within the ventral stream of visual cortices. In the monkey brain, this area receives strong feedforward connections from the primary visual cortex (V1) and sends strong projections to other secondary visual cortices (V3, V4, and V5).<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Stepniewska|first1=I|last2=Kaas|first2=J. H.|title=Topographic patterns of V2 cortical connections in macaque monkeys|journal=The Journal of Comparative Neurology|date=1996|volume=371|issue=1|pages=129–152|doi=10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960715)371:1<129::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-5|pmid=8835723}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gattas|first1=R|last2=Sousa|first2=A. P|last3=Mishkin|first3=M|last4=Ungerleider|first4=L. G.|title=Cortical projections of area V2 in the macaque|journal=Cerebral Cortex|date=1997|volume=7|issue=2|pages=110–129|doi=10.1093/cercor/7.2.110}}</ref> Most of the neurons of this area are tuned to simple visual characteristics such as orientation, spatial frequency, size, color, and shape.<ref name="A. Anzai, X. Peng 2007">{{cite journal|last1=Anzai|first1=A|last2=Peng|first2=X|last3=Van Essen|first3=D. C|title=Neurons in monkey visual area V2 encode combinations of orientations|journal=Nature Neuroscience|date=2007|volume=10|issue=10|pages=1313–21|pmid=17873872|doi=10.1038/nn1975}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hegdé|first1=Jay|last2=Van Essen|first2=D. C|title=Selectivity for Complex Shapes in Primate Visual Area V2|journal=The Journal of Neuroscience|date=2000|volume=20|url=http://www.jneurosci.org/content/20/5/RC61.short}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hegdé|first1=Jay|last2=Van Essen|first2=D. C|title=Temporal dynamics of shape analysis in Macaque visual area V2|journal=Journal of Psychology|date=2004|volume=92|issue=5|pages=3030–3042|doi=10.1152/jn.00822.2003}}</ref> Anatomical studies implicate layer 3 of area V2 in visual-information processing. In contrast to layer 3, layer 6 of the visual cortex is composed of many types of neurons, and their response to visual stimuli is more complex.
In a recent study, the Layer 6 cells of the V2 cortex were found to play a very important role in the storage of Object Recognition Memory as well as the conversion of short-term object memories into long-term memories.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=López-Aranda et alt.|title=Role of Layer 6 of V2 Visual Cortex in Object Recognition Memory|journal=Science|date=2009|volume=325|issue=5936|pages=87–89|doi=10.1126/science.1170869|pmid=19574389}}</ref>
== Third visual cortex, including area V3 ==
The term '''third visual complex''' refers to the region of cortex located immediately in front of V2, which includes the region named '''visual area V3''' in humans. The "complex" nomenclature is justified by the fact that some controversy still exists regarding the exact extent of area V3, with some researchers proposing that the cortex located in front of V2 may include two or three functional subdivisions. For example, David Van Essen and others (1986) have proposed the existence of a "dorsal V3" in the upper part of the cerebral hemisphere, which is distinct from the "ventral V3" (or ventral posterior area, VP) located in the lower part of the brain. Dorsal and ventral V3 have distinct connections with other parts of the brain, appear different in sections stained with a variety of methods, and contain neurons that respond to different combinations of visual stimulus (for example, colour-selective neurons are more common in the ventral V3). Additional subdivisions, including V3A and V3B have also been reported in humans. These subdivisions are located near dorsal V3, but do not adjoin V2.
Dorsal V3 is normally considered to be part of the dorsal stream, receiving inputs from V2 and from the primary visual area and projecting to the posterior [[parietal cortex]]. It may be anatomically located in [[Brodmann area 19]]. Braddick using fMRI has suggested that area V3/V3A may play a role in the processing of [[global motion]]<ref name="Braddick2001">{{cite journal | author=Braddick, OJ, O'Brien, JM| year=2001 | title=Brain areas sensitive to coherent visual motion | journal=Perception | volume=30 | pages=61–72 | doi = 10.1068/p3048 | pmid=11257978 | issue=1|display-authors=etal}}</ref> Other studies prefer to consider dorsal V3 as part of a larger area, named the [[dorsomedial area]] (DM), which contains a representation of the entire visual field. Neurons in area DM respond to [[coherent motion]] of large patterns covering extensive portions of the visual field (Lui and collaborators, 2006).
Ventral V3 (VP), has much weaker connections from the primary visual area, and stronger connections with the [[inferior temporal cortex]]. While earlier studies proposed that VP contained a representation of only the upper part of the visual field (above the point of fixation), more recent work indicates that this area is more extensive than previously appreciated, and like other visual areas it may contain a complete visual representation. The revised, more extensive VP is referred to as the ventrolateral posterior area (VLP) by Rosa and Tweedale.<ref>{{cite journal | doi = 10.1002/1096-9861(20000710)422:4<621::AID-CNE10>3.0.CO;2-E | last1 = Rosa | first1 = MG | last2 = Tweedale | first2 = R |name-list-format=vanc | year = 2000 | title = Visual areas in lateral and ventral extrastriate cortices of the marmoset monkey | url = | journal = J Comp Neurol | volume = 422 | issue = 4| pages = 621–51 | pmid = 10861530 }}</ref>
== V4 ==
{{See also|Color center}}
'''Visual area V4''' is one of the visual areas in the [[extrastriate]] visual cortex. In [[Rhesus macaque|macaques]], it is located anterior to V2 and posterior to [[Inferior temporal gyrus|posterior inferotemporal area (PIT)]]. It comprises at least four regions (left and right V4d, left and right V4v), and some groups report that it contains rostral and caudal subdivisions as well. It is unknown what the human [[homology (biology)|homologue]] of V4 is, and this issue is currently the subject of much scrutiny.<ref name="GoddardEtAl">{{cite journal | author=Goddard E | year=2011 | title=Color responsiveness argues against a dorsal component of human V4 | journal=Journal of Vision | volume=11 | issue=4 | pages=3 | doi = 10.1167/11.4.3 | last2=Goddard | first2=Erin | pmid=21467155 | last3=McDonald | first3=J. S. | last4=Solomon | first4=S. G. | last5=Clifford | first5=C. W. G.|display-authors=etal}}</ref>
V4 is the third cortical area in the [[Two Streams hypothesis#Ventral stream|ventral stream]], receiving strong feedforward input from V2 and sending strong connections to the [[Inferior temporal gyrus|PIT]]. It also receives direct input from V1, especially for central space. In addition, it has weaker connections to V5 and [[Angular gyrus|dorsal prelunate gyrus]] (DP).
V4 is the first area in the [[Two Streams hypothesis#Ventral stream|ventral stream]] to show strong attentional modulation. Most studies indicate that [[selective attention]] can change firing rates in V4 by about 20%. A seminal paper by Moran and Desimone characterizing these effects was the first paper to find attention effects anywhere in the visual cortex.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Moran|first=J|last2= Desimone|first2=R|title=Selective Attention Gates Visual Processing in the Extrastriate Cortex|journal=Science|date=1985|volume=229|issue=4715|pages=782–4|doi=10.1126/science.4023713|pmid=4023713}}</ref>
Like V2, V4 is tuned for orientation, spatial frequency, and color. Unlike V2, V4 is tuned for object features of intermediate complexity, like simple geometric shapes, although no one has developed a full parametric description of the tuning space for V4. Visual area V4 is not tuned for complex objects such as faces, as areas in the [[Inferior temporal gyrus|inferotemporal cortex]] are.
The firing properties of V4 were first described by [[Semir Zeki]] in the late 1970s, who also named the area. Before that, V4 was known by its anatomical description, the [[prelunate gyrus]]. Originally, Zeki argued that the purpose of V4 was to process color information. Work in the early 1980s proved that V4 was as directly involved in form recognition as earlier cortical areas.{{citation needed|date=March 2016}} This research supported the [[Two Streams hypothesis]], first presented by Ungerleider and Mishkin in 1982.
Recent work has shown that V4 exhibits long-term plasticity,<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Schmid|first1=M. C.|last2=Schmiedt|first2=J. T.|last3=Peters|first3=A. J.|last4=Saunders|first4=R. C.|last5=Maier|first5=A.|last6=Leopold|first6=D. A.|title=Motion-Sensitive Responses in Visual Area V4 in the Absence of Primary Visual Cortex|journal=Journal of Neuroscience|date=27 November 2013|volume=33|issue=48|pages=18740–18745|doi=10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3923-13.2013|url=http://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneuro/33/48/18740.full.pdf}}</ref> encodes stimulus salience, is gated by signals coming from the [[frontal eye fields]],<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Moore|first=Tirin|last2=Armstrong|first2=Katherine M.|title=Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex|url=http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nature01341|journal=Nature|volume=421|issue=6921|pages=370–373|doi=10.1038/nature01341}}</ref> and shows changes in the spatial profile of its receptive fields with attention.{{citation needed|date=March 2016}}
== Middle temporal visual area (V5) ==<!-- This section is linked from [[V5]] -->
The '''middle temporal visual area''' ('''MT''' or '''V5''') is a region of extrastriate visual cortex. In several species of both [[New World monkey]]s and [[Old World monkey]]s the MT area contains a high concentration of direction-selective neurons.<ref name="BornBradley" /> The MT in primates is thought to play a major role in the [[motion perception|perception of motion]], the integration of local motion signals into global percepts, and the guidance of some [[Eye movement (sensory)|eye movements]].<ref name="BornBradley">{{cite journal |vauthors=Born R, Bradley D | title = Structure and function of visual area MT | journal = Annu Rev Neurosci | volume = 28 | issue = | pages = 157–89 | year = 2005| pmid = 16022593 | doi = 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131052}}</ref>
=== Connections ===
MT is connected to a wide array of cortical and subcortical brain areas. Its input comes from visual cortical areas V1, V2 and dorsal V3 ([[dorsomedial area]]),<ref name="FellemanVanEssen">{{cite journal |vauthors=Felleman D, Van Essen D | title = Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex | journal = [[Cereb Cortex]] | volume = 1 | issue = 1 | pages = 1–47 | year = 1991| pmid = 1822724 | doi = 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a}}</ref><ref name="UngerleiderDesimone">{{cite journal |vauthors=Ungerleider L, Desimone R | title = Cortical connections of visual area MT in the macaque | journal = J Comp Neurol | volume = 248 | issue = 2 | pages = 190–222 | year = 1986 | pmid = 3722458 | doi = 10.1002/cne.902480204}}</ref> the [[koniocellular]] regions of the [[LGN]],<ref name="Sincich">{{cite journal |vauthors=Sincich L, Park K, Wohlgemuth M, Horton J | title = Bypassing V1: a direct geniculate input to area MT | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 7 | issue = 10 | pages = 1123–8 | year = 2004 | pmid = 15378066 | doi = 10.1038/nn1318}}</ref> and the [[pulvinar nuclei|inferior pulvinar]].<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Warner CE, Goldshmit Y, Bourne JA | title = Retinal afferents synapse with relay cells targeting the middle temporal area in the pulvinar and lateral geniculate nuclei | journal = Front Neuroanat | volume = 4 | pages = 8 | year = 2010 | pmid = 20179789 | doi = 10.3389/neuro.05.008.2010 | pmc=2826187}}</ref> The pattern of projections to MT changes somewhat between the representations of the foveal and peripheral visual fields, with the latter receiving inputs from areas located in the midline cortex and [[retrosplenial region]].<ref name="PalmerRosa2006">{{cite journal |vauthors=Palmer SM, Rosa MG | year=2006 | title=A distinct anatomical network of cortical areas for analysis of motion in far peripheral vision | journal=Eur J Neurosci | volume=24 |pages=2389–405 | doi=10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05113.x | pmid=17042793 | issue=8}}</ref>
A standard view is that V1 provides the "most important" input to MT.<ref name="BornBradley" /> Nonetheless, several studies have demonstrated that neurons in MT are capable of responding to visual information, often in a direction-selective manner, even after V1 has been destroyed or inactivated.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rodman | first1 = HR | last2 = Gross | first2 = CG | last3 = Albright | first3 = TD |name-list-format=vanc | year = 1989 | title = Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. I. Effects of striate cortex removal | url = | journal = J Neurosci | volume = 9 | issue = 6| pages = 2033–50 | pmid = 2723765 }}</ref> Moreover, research by [[Semir Zeki]] and collaborators has suggested that certain types of visual information may reach MT before it even reaches V1.
MT sends its major output to areas located in the cortex immediately surrounding it, including areas FST, MST, and V4t (middle temporal crescent). Other projections of MT target the eye movement-related areas of the frontal and parietal lobes (frontal eye field and lateral intraparietal area).
=== Function ===
The first studies of the [[electrophysiological]] properties of neurons in MT showed that a large portion of the cells are [[neuronal tuning|tuned]] to the speed and direction of moving visual stimuli.<ref name="DubnerZeki">{{cite journal |vauthors=Dubner R, Zeki S | title = Response properties and receptive fields of cells in an anatomically defined region of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey | journal = Brain Res | volume = 35 | issue = 2 | pages = 528–32 | year = 1971 | pmid = 5002708 | doi = 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90494-X}}.</ref><ref name="MaunsellVanEssen">{{cite journal |vauthors=Maunsell J, Van Essen D | title = Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation | journal = J Neurophysiol | volume = 49 | issue = 5 | pages = 1127–47 | year = 1983 | pmid = 6864242}}</ref>
[[Lesion]] studies have also supported the role of MT in motion perception and eye movements.<ref name=Dursteler1987>{{cite journal | title=Directional pursuit deficits following lesions of the foveal representation within the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey |author1=Dursteler M.R. |author2=Wurtz R.H. |author3=Newsome W.T. | journal=Journal of Neurophysiology | year=1987 | volume=57 | issue=5 | pages=1262–87 | pmid=3585468}}</ref> [[Neuropsychology|Neuropsychological]] studies of a patient unable to see motion, seeing the world in a series of static 'frames' instead, suggested that V5 in the primate is homologous to MT in the human.<ref name=Hess1989>{{cite journal | title=The 'motion-blind' patient: low-level spatial and temporal filters |author1=Hess R.H. |author2=Baker C.L. |author3=Zihl J. | journal=Journal of Neuroscience | year=1989 | volume=9 | issue=5 | pages=1628–40 | pmid=2723744}}</ref><ref name=Baker1991>{{cite journal | title=Residual motion perception in a 'motion-blind' patient, assessed with limited-lifetime random dot stimuli |author1=Baker C.L. Jr |author2=Hess R.F |author3=Zihl J. | journal=Journal of Neuroscience | year=1991 | volume=11 | issue=2 | pages=454–61 | pmid=1992012}}</ref>
However, since neurons in V1 are also tuned to the direction and speed of motion, these early results left open the question of precisely what MT could do that V1 could not. Much work has been carried out on this region, as it appears to integrate local visual motion signals into the global motion of complex objects.<ref name="Movshon">Movshon, J.A., Adelson, E.H., Gizzi, M.S., & Newsome, W.T. (1985). The analysis of moving visual patterns. In: C. Chagas, R. Gattass, & C. Gross (Eds.), Pattern recognition mechanisms (pp. 117-151), Rome: Vatican Press.</ref>
For example, ''lesion'' to the V5 leads to deficits in perceiving motion and processing of complex stimuli. It contains many neurons selective for the motion of complex visual features (line ends, corners). ''Microstimulation'' of a neuron located in the V5 affects the perception of motion. For example, if one finds a neuron with preference for upward motion in a monkey's V5 and stimulates it with an electrode, then the monkey becomes more likely to report 'upward' motion when presented with stimuli containing 'left' and 'right' as well as 'upward' components.<ref name="BrittenVanWezel">{{cite journal | title=Electrical microstimulation of cortical area MST biases heading perception in monkeys |author1=Britten K.H. |author2=van Wezel R.J. | journal=Nat. Neurosci. | year=1998 | volume=1 | issue=1 | pages=59–63 | pmid=10195110 | doi=10.1038/259}}</ref>
There is still much controversy over the exact form of the computations carried out in area MT<ref name="Wilson">{{cite journal | last1 = Wilson | first1 = H.R. | last2 = Ferrera | first2 = V.P. | last3 = Yo | first3 = C. | year = 1992 | title = A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception | url = | journal = Vis Neurosci | volume = 9 | issue = 1| pages = 79–97 | doi=10.1017/s0952523800006386}}</ref> and some research suggests that feature motion is in fact already available at lower levels of the visual system such as V1.
<ref name="Tinsley">{{cite journal | title=The nature of V1 neural responses to 2D moving patterns depends on receptive-field structure in the marmoset monkey | author=Tinsley, C.J., Webb, B.S., Barraclough, N.E., Vincent, C.J., Parker, A., & Derrington, A.M.| journal=J Neurophysiol | year=2003 | volume=90 | issue=2 | pages=930–7 | pmid=12711710 | doi=10.1152/jn.00708.2002}}</ref>
<ref name="PackBorn">{{cite journal | title=Two-dimensional substructure of stereo and motion interactions in macaque visual cortex |author1=Pack C.C. |author2=Born R.T. |author3=Livingstone M.S. | journal=Neuron | year=2003 | volume=37 | issue=3 | pages=525–35 | pmid=12575958 | doi=10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01187-x}}</ref>
=== Functional organization ===
MT was shown to be organized in direction columns.<ref name="Albright">{{cite journal | author = Albright T | title = Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque | journal = J Neurophysiol | volume = 52 | issue = 6 | pages = 1106–30 | year = 1984 | pmid = 6520628}}</ref> DeAngelis argued that MT neurons were also organized based on their tuning for binocular disparity.<ref name="DeAngelisNewsome">{{cite journal |vauthors=DeAngelis G, Newsome W | title = Organization of disparity-selective neurons in macaque area MT | journal = J Neurosci | volume = 19 | issue = 4 | pages = 1398–415 | year = 1999 | pmid = 9952417}}</ref>
==V6==
<!-- [[Dorsomedial area]] redirects to this section -->
The '''dorsomedial area''' (DM) also known as '''V6''', appears to respond to visual stimuli associated with self-motion <ref>{{cite journal|last1=Cardin|first1=V|last2=Smith|first2=AT|title=Sensitivity of human visual and vestibular cortical regions to stereoscopic depth gradients associated with self-motion|date=2010|volume=20|issue=8|pages=1964–73|doi=10.1093/cercor/bhp268|journal=Cerebral Cortex}}</ref> and wide-field stimulation.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Pitzalis et alt.|title=Wide-Field Retinotopy Defines Human Cortical Visual Area V6|journal=The Journal of Neuroscience|date=2006|volume=26|issue=30|pages=7962–73|pmid=16870741|doi=10.1523/jneurosci.0178-06.2006}}</ref> V6, is a subdivision of the visual cortex of primates first described by [[John Allman]] and [[Jon Kaas]] in 1975.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Allman JM, Kaas JH | year = 1975 | title = The dorsomedial cortical visual area: a third tier area in the occipital lobe of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus) | url = | journal = Brain Res | volume = 100 | issue = 3| pages = 473–487 | doi=10.1016/0006-8993(75)90153-5}}</ref> V6 is located in the dorsal part of the [[extrastriate cortex]], near the deep groove through the centre of the brain ([[medial longitudinal fissure]]), and typically also includes portions of the medial cortex, such as the [[parieto-occipital sulcus]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} DM contains a topographically organized representation of the entire field of vision.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
There are similarities between the visual area V5 and V6 of the [[common marmoset]]. Both areas receive direct connections from the [[primary visual cortex]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} And both have a high [[myelin]] content, a characteristic that is usually present in brain structures involved in fast transmission of information.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
For many years, it was considered that DM only existed in [[New World monkeys]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} However, more recent research has suggested that DM also exists in [[Old World monkeys]] and perhaps humans.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} V6 is also sometimes referred to as the parieto-occipital area (PO), although the correspondence is not exact.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Galletti C, etal | year = 2005 | title = The relationship between V6 and PO in macaque extrastriate cortex | url = http://www.gallettilab.unibo.it/Pdf/Galletti et al 2005.pdf| journal = Eur J Neurosci | volume = 21 | issue = | pages = 959–970 | doi=10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03911.x}}</ref><ref name="Gal">{{cite journal |vauthors=Galletti C, etal | year = 2003 | title = Role of the medial parieto-occipital cortex in the control of reaching and grasping movements | url = | journal = Exp Brain Res | volume = 153 | issue = | pages = 158–170 | doi=10.1007/s00221-003-1589-z}}</ref>
===Properties===
Neurons in area DM/V6 of [[night monkey]]s and [[common marmoset]]s have unique response properties, including an extremely sharp selectivity for the orientation of visual contours, and preference for long, uninterrupted lines covering large parts of the visual field.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Baker JF, etal | year = 1981 | title = Visual response properties of neurons in four extrastriate visual areas of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus): a quantitative comparison of medial, dorsomedial, dorsolateral, and middle temporal areas | url = | journal = J Neurophysiol | volume = 45 | issue = | pages = 397–416 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Lui LL, etal | year = 2006 | title = Functional response properties of neurons in the dorsomedial visual area of New World monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) | url = | journal = Cereb Cortex | volume = 16 | issue = 2| pages = 162–177 | doi=10.1093/cercor/bhi094}}</ref>
However, in comparison with area MT, a much smaller proportion of DM cells shows selectivity for the direction of motion of visual patterns.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} Another notable difference with area MT is that cells in DM are attuned to low spatial frequency components of an image, and respond poorly to the motion of textured patterns such as a field of random dots.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} These response properties suggest that DM and MT may work in parallel, with the former analyzing self-motion relative to the environment, and the latter analyzing the motion of individual objects relative to the background.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
Recently, an area responsive to wide-angle flow fields has been identified in the human and is thought to be a homologue of macaque area V6.<ref>{{cite journal | authors=Pitzalis, S., Sereno, M.I., Committeri, G., Fattori, P., Galati, G., Patria, F., & Galletti, C. | year=2010 | title=Human v6: The medial motion area. | journal=Cereb Cortex | volume=20 | issue=2 | pages=411–424 | doi=10.1093/cercor/bhp112}}</ref>
===Pathways===
The connections and response properties of cells in DM/ V6 suggest that this area is a key node in a subset of the '[[dorsal stream]]', referred to by some as the 'dorsomedial pathway'.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} This pathway is likely to be important for the control of skeletomotor activity, including postural reactions and reaching movements towards objects <ref name="Gal"/> The main 'feedforward' connection of DM is to the cortex immediately rostral to it, in the interface between the occipital and parietal lobes (V6A).{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} This region has, in turn, relatively direct connections with the regions of the frontal lobe that control arm movements, including the [[premotor cortex]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
== See also ==
{{Portal|Neuroscience}}
* [[Cortical area]]
* [[Cortical blindness]]
* [[Feature integration theory]]
* [[List of regions in the human brain]]
* [[Retinotopy]]
* [[Visual processing]]
* [[Visual feature array]]
* [[Complex cell]]
==References==
{{Reflist|30em}}
==External links==
{{Commons category|Visual cortex}}
* [http://webvision.med.utah.edu/VisualCortex.html The Primary Visual Cortex by Matthew Schmolesky] at [[University of Utah]]
* [http://hubel.med.harvard.edu/book/b23.htm Architecture of the Visual Cortex, by David Hubel] at [[Harvard University]]
* {{BrainInfo|ancil|415}} - striate area 17
* {{BrainInfo|ancil|699}} - Brodmann area 17 in guenon
* {{BrainMaps|visual%20cortex}}
* [http://topographica.org Simulator for computational modeling of visual cortex maps] at topographica.org
{{Prosencephalon}}
{{Visual system}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Visual Cortex}}
[[Category:Visual cortex]]
[[Category:Visual perception]]' |
New page wikitext, after the edit (new_wikitext ) | '<!-- This page is highly dated and has a lot of archaic jargon; please clean it! -->
{{Infobox brain
| Name = Visual cortex
| Latin = Cortex visualis
| GraySubject =
| GrayPage =
| Image = Brodmann areas 17 18 19.png
| Caption = View of the brain from behind. Red = Brodmann area 17 (primary visual cortex); orange = area 18; yellow = area 19
| Image2 = Brodmann Cytoarchitectonics 17.png
| Caption2 = Brain shown from the side, facing left. Above: view from outside, below: cut through the middle. Orange = Brodmann area 17 (primary visual cortex)
| IsPartOf =
| Components =
| Artery =
| Vein =
| BrainInfoType =
| BrainInfoNumber =
| MeshName =
| MeshNumber =
| DorlandsPre = c_57
| DorlandsSuf = 12261838
|NeuroLex = Visual cortex primary
|NeuroLexID = nlx_143552
}}The '''visual cortex''' of the [[brain]] is a part of the [[cerebral cortex]] that processes [[visual perception|visual information]]. It is located in the [[occipital lobe]] in the back of the head.
Visual information coming from the eye goes through the [[lateral geniculate nucleus]] in the [[thalamus]] and then reaches the visual cortex. The part of the visual cortex that receives the sensory inputs from the [[thalamus]] is the primary visual cortex, also known as visual area 1 (V1), and the striate cortex. The extrastriate areas consist of visual areas 2 (V2), 3 (V3), 4 (V4), and 5 (V5).<ref>{{cite web|last1=Mather|first1=George|title=The Visual Cortex|url=http://www.lifesci.sussex.ac.uk/home/George_Mather/Linked%20Pages/Physiol/Cortex.html|website=School of Life Sciences: University of Sussex|publisher=University of Sussex|accessdate=6 March 2017|language=English}}</ref>
Both [[cerebral hemisphere|hemispheres of the brain]] contain a visual cortex; the visual cortex in the left hemisphere receives signals from the right [[visual field]], and the visual cortex in the right hemisphere receives signals from the left visual field.
==Introduction==
The primary visual cortex (V1) is located in and around the [[calcarine fissure]] in the [[occipital lobe]]. Each hemisphere's V1 receives information directly from its ipsilateral [[lateral geniculate nucleus]] that receives signals from the contralateral visual hemifield.
Neurons in the visual cortex fire [[action potential]]s when visual stimuli appear within their [[receptive field]]. By definition, the receptive field is the region within the entire visual field that elicits an [[action potential]]. But, for any given neuron, it may respond best to a subset of stimuli within its receptive field. This property is called ''[[neuronal tuning]]''. In the earlier visual areas, neurons have simpler tuning. For example, a neuron in V1 may fire to any vertical stimulus in its receptive field. In the higher visual areas, neurons have complex tuning. For example, in the inferior temporal cortex (IT), a neuron may fire only when a certain face appears in its receptive field.
The visual cortex receives its blood supply primarily from the [[calcarine artery|calcarine branch]] of the [[posterior cerebral artery]].
[[File:Neural pathway diagram.svg|thumb]]
== Psychological model of the neural processing of visual information ==
{{Main article|Two-streams hypothesis}}
=== Ventral-dorsal model ===
[[File:Ventral-dorsal streams.svg|thumb|right|300px|The [[dorsal stream]] (green) and [[ventral stream]] (purple) are shown. They originate from primary visual cortex.]]
Each V1 transmits information to two primary pathways, called the ventral stream and the dorsal stream.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
* The '''[[Two Streams hypothesis#Ventral stream|ventral stream]]''' begins with V1, goes through visual area V2, then through visual area V4, and to the [[inferior temporal cortex]] (IT cortex). The ventral stream, sometimes called the "What Pathway", is associated with form recognition and object representation. It is also associated with storage of [[long-term memory]].
* The '''[[Two Streams hypothesis#Dorsal stream|dorsal stream]]''' begins with V1, goes through Visual area V2, then to the [[dorsomedial area]] (DM/ V6) and Visual area MT (middle temporal/ V5) and to the [[posterior parietal cortex]]. The dorsal stream, sometimes called the "Where Pathway" or "How Pathway", is associated with motion, representation of object locations, and control of the eyes and arms, especially when visual information is used to guide [[saccade]]s or reaching.
The what vs. where account of the ventral/dorsal pathways was first described by [[Leslie Ungerleider|Ungerleider]] and [[Mortimer Mishkin|Mishkin]].<ref name="UngerleiderMishkin82">{{cite book|title=Analysis of Visual Behavior|vauthors=Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M|publisher=MIT Press|year=1982|veditors=Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJ|location=Boston|pages=549–586|chapter=Two Cortical Visual Systems}}</ref>
More recently, [[Melvyn A. Goodale|Goodale]] and Milner extended these ideas and suggested that the ventral stream is critical for visual perception whereas the dorsal stream mediates the visual control of skilled actions.<ref name="GoodaleMilner">{{cite journal|year=1992|title=Separate pathways for perception and action.|journal=Trends in Neurosciences|volume=15|issue=1|pages=20–25|doi=10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8|pmid=1374953|vauthors=Goodale MA, Milner AD}}</ref> It has been shown that visual illusions such as the [[Ebbinghaus illusion]] distort judgements of a perceptual nature, but when the subject responds with an action, such as grasping, no distortion occurs.<ref name="Aglioti1995">{{cite journal|year=1995|title=Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand.|journal=Curr. Biol.|volume=5|issue=6|pages=679–85|doi=10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3|pmid=7552179|vauthors=Aglioti S, DeSouza JF, Goodale MA}}</ref>
Work such as the one from Scharnowski and Gegenfurtner<ref name="Franz2005">{{cite journal|year=2005|title=Illusion effects on grasping are temporally constant not dynamic.|journal=J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform.|volume=31|issue=6|pages=1359–78|doi=10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1359|pmid=16366795|author=Franz VH, Scharnowski F, Gegenfurtner}}</ref> suggests that both the action and perception systems are equally fooled by such illusions. Other studies, however, provide strong support for the idea that skilled actions such as grasping are not affected by pictorial illusions<ref name="Ganel2003">{{cite journal|year=2003|title=Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape.|journal=Nature|volume=426|issue=6967|pages=664–7|doi=10.1038/nature02156|pmid=14668865|vauthors=Ganel T, Goodale MA}}</ref><ref name="Ganel2008">{{cite journal|year=2008|title=A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: opposite effects of real and illusory size.|journal=Psych. Sci.|volume=19|issue=3|pages=221–5|doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02071.x|pmid=18315792|vauthors=Ganel T, Tanzer M, Goodale MA}}</ref> and suggest that the action/perception dissociation is a useful way to characterize the functional division of labor between the dorsal and ventral visual pathways in the cerebral cortex.<ref name="Goodale2011">{{cite journal|year=2011|title=Transforming vision into action.|journal=Vision Res.|volume=51|issue=14|pages=1567–87|doi=10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.027|pmid=20691202|author=Goodale MA.}}</ref>
== Primary visual cortex (V1)==
{{Refimprove section|date=November 2016}}
[[File:visualcortex.gif|right]]
[[File:Visual cortex - low mag.jpg|thumb|righ|[[Micrograph]] showing the visual cortex (pink). The [[pia mater]] and [[arachnoid mater]] including [[blood vessel]]s are seen at the top of the image. Subcortical [[white matter]] (blue) is seen at the bottom of the image. [[LFB stain|HE-LFB stain]].]]
The '''primary visual cortex''' is the most studied visual area in the brain. In mammals, it is located in the posterior pole of the occipital lobe and is the simplest, earliest cortical visual area. It is highly specialized for processing information about static and moving objects and is excellent in [[pattern recognition]].{{Clarify|reason=unclear what "highly specialized" and "excellent" means in this sentence.|date=November 2016}}
The functionally defined primary visual cortex is approximately equivalent to the anatomically defined '''striate cortex'''.{{Clarify|date=November 2016}} The name "striate cortex" is derived from the [[line of Gennari]], a distinctive stripe visible to the naked eye<ref>Glickstein M., Rizzolatti G. ''Francesco Gennari and the structure of the cerebral cortex'' Trends in Neurosciences, Volume 7, Issue 12, 464-467, 1 December 1984.</ref> that represents [[myelin]]ated [[axons]] from the [[lateral geniculate body]] terminating in layer 4 of the [[gray matter]].
The primary visual cortex is divided into six functionally distinct layers, labeled 1 to 6. Layer 4, which receives most visual input from the [[lateral geniculate nucleus]] (LGN), is further divided into 4 layers, labelled 4A, 4B, 4Cα, and 4Cβ. Sublamina 4Cα{{Clarify|reason=unclear what a sublamina is|date=November 2016}} receives mostly [[Magnocellular cell|magnocellular]] input from the LGN, while layer 4Cβ receives input from [[Parvocellular cell|parvocellular]] pathways.
The average number of neurons in the adult human primary visual cortex in each hemisphere has been estimated at around 140 million.<ref name="Leuba-Kraftsik-1994">{{cite journal |author1= Leuba G |author2= Kraftsik R |year= 1994 |title=Changes in volume, surface estimate, three-dimensional shape and total number of neurons of the human primary visual cortex from midgestation until old age |journal=Anatomy and Embryology |volume=190 |issue=4 |pages=351–366 |publisher= |doi= 10.1007/BF00187293|pmid=7840422 |pmc= |url= |accessdate=}}</ref>
=== Function ===
{{relevance inline|date=September 2016}}
{{technical|date=September 2016}}
V1 has a very well-defined map of the spatial information in vision. For example, in humans, the upper bank of the [[calcarine sulcus]] responds strongly to the lower half of [[visual field]] (below the center), and the lower bank of the calcarine to the upper half of visual field. In concept, this [[retinotopy|retinotopic]] mapping is a transformation of the visual image from [[retina]] to V1. The correspondence between a given location in V1 and in the subjective visual field is very precise: even the [[Blind spot (vision)|blind spots]] are mapped into V1. In terms of evolution, this correspondence is very basic and found in most animals that possess a V1. In humans and animals with a [[Fovea centralis|fovea]] in the retina, a large portion of V1 is mapped to the small, central portion of visual field, a phenomenon known as [[cortical magnification]].<ref>{{cite book|last1=Barghout|first1=Lauren|title=On the Differences Between Peripheral and Foveal Pattern Masking|date=1999|publisher=Masters Thesis. U.C. Berkeley|location=Berkeley, California, U.S.A.}}</ref> Perhaps for the purpose of accurate spatial encoding, neurons in V1 have the smallest [[receptive field]] size of any visual cortex microscopic regions.
The tuning properties of V1 neurons (what the neurons respond to) differ greatly over time. Early in time (40 ms and further) individual V1 neurons have strong tuning to a small set of stimuli. That is, the neuronal responses can discriminate small changes in visual [[Orientation (mental)|orientations]], [[spatial frequencies]] and [[color]]s. Furthermore, individual V1 neurons in human and animals with [[binocular vision]] have ocular dominance, namely tuning to one of the two eyes. In V1, and primary sensory cortex in general, neurons with similar tuning properties tend to cluster together as [[cortical column]]s. [[David Hubel]] and [[Torsten Wiesel]] proposed the classic ice-cube organization model of cortical columns for two tuning properties: [[ocular dominance columns|ocular dominance]] and orientation. However, this model cannot accommodate the color, spatial frequency and many other features to which neurons are tuned {{Citation needed|date=November 2011}}. The exact organization of all these cortical columns within V1 remains a hot topic of current research. The mathematical modeling of this function has been compared to [[Gabor transform]]s.
Later in time (after 100 ms), neurons in V1 are also sensitive to the more global organisation of the scene (Lamme & Roelfsema, 2000).<ref>{{cite book|last1=Barghout|first1=Lauren|title=Vision: How Global Perceptual Context Changes Local Contrast Processing (Ph.D. Dissertation). Updated to include computer vision techniques|date=2003|publisher=Scholar's Press|isbn=978-3-639-70962-9|url=https://www.morebooks.de/store/gb/book/vision/isbn/978-3-639-70962-9}}</ref> These response properties probably stem from recurrent [[feedback]] processing (the influence of higher-tier cortical areas on lower-tier cortical areas) and lateral connections from [[Pyramidal cell|pyramidal neurons]] (Hupe et al. 1998). While feedforward connections are mainly driving, feedback connections are mostly modulatory in their effects (Angelucci et al., 2003; Hupe et al., 2001). Evidence shows that feedback originating in higher-level areas such as V4, IT, or MT, with bigger and more complex receptive fields, can modify and shape V1 responses, accounting for contextual or [[extra-classical receptive field]] effects (Guo et al., 2007; Huang et al., 2007; Sillito et al., 2006).
The visual information relayed to V1 is not coded in terms of spatial (or optical) imagery but rather are better described as [[edge detection]]. As an example, for an image comprising half side black and half side white, the dividing line between black and white has strongest local contrast (that is, edge detection) and is encoded, while few neurons code the brightness information (black or white per se). As information is further relayed to subsequent visual areas, it is coded as increasingly non-local frequency/phase signals. Note that, at these early stages of cortical visual processing, spatial location of visual information is well preserved amid the local contrast encoding (edge detection).
Axiomatically{{clarify|date=September 2016}} determined functional models of simple cells in V1 have been determined by Lindeberg<ref name=Lin13BICY>{{cite journal | last1 = Lindeberg | first1 = T. | year = | title = A computational theory of visual receptive fields | doi = 10.1007/s00422-013-0569-z | journal = Biological Cybernetics | volume = 107 | issue = 6| pages = 589–635 }}</ref><ref name=Lin16-JMIV>{{cite journal | last1 = Lindeberg | first1 = T. | year = | title = Time-causal and time-recursive spatio-temporal receptive fields | url = http://www.csc.kth.se/~tony/abstracts/Lin16-JMIV.html | journal = Journal of Mathematical Imaging and Vision | volume = 55 | issue = 1| pages = 50–88 | doi=10.1007/s10851-015-0613-9}}</ref> in terms of directional derivatives{{clarify|date=September 2016}} of affine Gaussian kernels{{clarify|date=September 2016}} over the spatial domain{{clarify|date=September 2016}} in combination with temporal derivatives{{clarify|date=September 2016}} of either non-causal or time-causal scale-space kernels{{clarify|date=September 2016}} over the temporal domain (see [[axiomatic theory of receptive fields]]). Specifically, it has been shown that this theory both leads to predictions about receptive fields with good qualitative agreement with the biological receptive field measurements performed by DeAngelis et al.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = DeAngelis | first1 = G. C. | last2 = Ohzawa | first2 = I. | last3 = Freeman | first3 = R. D. | year = 1995 | title = Receptive field dynamics in the central visual pathways | url = | journal = Trends Neurosci | volume = 18 | issue = 10| pages = 451–457 | doi=10.1016/0166-2236(95)94496-r | pmid=8545912}}</ref><ref>G. C. DeAngelis and A. Anzai "A modern view of the classical receptive field: linear and non-linear spatio-temporal processing by V1 neurons. In: Chalupa, L.M., Werner, J.S. (eds.) The Visual Neurosciences, vol. 1, pp. 704–719. MIT Press, Cambridge, 2004.</ref> and guarantees good theoretical properties of the mathematical receptive field model, including covariance and invariance properties under natural image transformations.<ref name=Lin13PONE>{{cite journal | last1 = Lindeberg | first1 = T. | year = | title = Invariance of visual operations at the level of receptive fields | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0066990 | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 8 | issue = 7| page = e66990 }}</ref>{{relevance inline|date=September 2016}}
Differences in size of V1 also seem to have an effect on the [[Interindividual differences in perception|perception of illusions]].<ref name="surface_V1">{{cite journal | url=http://www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v14/n1/full/nn.2706.html | title=The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size | author=Schwarzkopf, D Samuel | journal=Nature Neuroscience | volume=14 | year=2011 | issue=1 | pages=28–30 | doi=10.1038/nn.2706 | pmid=21131954 | pmc=3012031}}</ref>
== V2 == <!--Please keep this header, as it is a redirect -->
'''Visual area V2''', or '''secondary visual cortex''', also called '''prestriate cortex''',<ref>Gazzaniga, Ivry & Mangun: ''Cognitive neuroscience,'' 2002</ref> is the second major area in the visual cortex, and the first region within the '''visual association area'''. It receives strong [[Feedforward neural network|feedforward]] connections from V1 (direct and via the pulvinar) and sends strong connections to V3, V4, and V5. It also sends strong [[feedback]] connections to V1.
In terms of anatomy, V2 is split into four quadrants, a [[Dorsum (biology)|dorsal]] and [[ventral]] representation in the left and the right [[cerebral hemisphere|hemispheres]]. Together, these four regions provide a complete map of the visual world. V2 has many properties in common with V1: Cells are tuned to simple properties such as orientation, spatial frequency, and colour. The responses of many V2 neurons are also modulated by more complex properties, such as the orientation of [[illusory contours]],<ref name="illusory contours">{{cite journal|last1=von der Heydt|first1=R|last2=Peterhans|first2=E|last3=Baumgartner|first3=G|title=Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses|journal=Science|date=1984|volume=224|pages=1260–62|doi=10.1126/science.6539501|pmid=6539501}}</ref><ref name="A. Anzai, X. Peng 2007"/> [[binocular disparity]],<ref name="stereoscopic edges">{{cite journal|last1=von der Heydt|first1=R|last2=Zhou|first2=H|last3=Friedman|first3=H. S|title=Representation of stereoscopic edges in monkey visual cortex|journal=Vision Research|date=2000|volume=40|pages=1955–67|doi=10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00044-4}}</ref> and whether the stimulus is part of the figure or the ground.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Qiu|first1=F. T|last2=von der Heydt|first2=R|title=Figure and ground in the visual cortex: V2 combines stereoscopic cues with Gestalt rules|journal=Neuron|date=2005|volume=47|pages=155–66|url=https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.028|doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.028}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Maruko, I; et alt.|title=Postnatal Development of Disparity Sensitivity in Visual Area 2 (V2) of Macaque Monkeys|journal=Journal of Neurophysiology|date=2008|volume=100|issue=5|pages=2486–2495|doi=10.1152/jn.90397.2008|pmid=18753321|pmc=2585398}}</ref> Recent research has shown that V2 cells show a small amount of attentional modulation (more than V1, less than V4), are tuned for moderately complex patterns, and may be driven by multiple orientations at different subregions within a single receptive field.
It is argued that the entire ventral visual-to-hippocampal stream is important for visual memory.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Bussey|first1=T J|last2=Saksida|first2=L. M|title=Memory, perception, and the ventral visual-perirhinal-hippocampal stream: thinking outside of the boxes|journal=Hippocampus|date=2007|volume=17|issue=9|pages = 898–908|doi=10.1002/hipo.20320|pmid=17636546}}</ref> This theory, unlike the dominant one, predicts that object-recognition memory (ORM) alterations could result from the manipulation in V2, an area that is highly interconnected within the ventral stream of visual cortices. In the monkey brain, this area receives strong feedforward connections from the primary visual cortex (V1) and sends strong projections to other secondary visual cortices (V3, V4, and V5).<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Stepniewska|first1=I|last2=Kaas|first2=J. H.|title=Topographic patterns of V2 cortical connections in macaque monkeys|journal=The Journal of Comparative Neurology|date=1996|volume=371|issue=1|pages=129–152|doi=10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960715)371:1<129::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-5|pmid=8835723}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gattas|first1=R|last2=Sousa|first2=A. P|last3=Mishkin|first3=M|last4=Ungerleider|first4=L. G.|title=Cortical projections of area V2 in the macaque|journal=Cerebral Cortex|date=1997|volume=7|issue=2|pages=110–129|doi=10.1093/cercor/7.2.110}}</ref> Most of the neurons of this area are tuned to simple visual characteristics such as orientation, spatial frequency, size, color, and shape.<ref name="A. Anzai, X. Peng 2007">{{cite journal|last1=Anzai|first1=A|last2=Peng|first2=X|last3=Van Essen|first3=D. C|title=Neurons in monkey visual area V2 encode combinations of orientations|journal=Nature Neuroscience|date=2007|volume=10|issue=10|pages=1313–21|pmid=17873872|doi=10.1038/nn1975}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hegdé|first1=Jay|last2=Van Essen|first2=D. C|title=Selectivity for Complex Shapes in Primate Visual Area V2|journal=The Journal of Neuroscience|date=2000|volume=20|url=http://www.jneurosci.org/content/20/5/RC61.short}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Hegdé|first1=Jay|last2=Van Essen|first2=D. C|title=Temporal dynamics of shape analysis in Macaque visual area V2|journal=Journal of Psychology|date=2004|volume=92|issue=5|pages=3030–3042|doi=10.1152/jn.00822.2003}}</ref> Anatomical studies implicate layer 3 of area V2 in visual-information processing. In contrast to layer 3, layer 6 of the visual cortex is composed of many types of neurons, and their response to visual stimuli is more complex.
In a recent study, the Layer 6 cells of the V2 cortex were found to play a very important role in the storage of Object Recognition Memory as well as the conversion of short-term object memories into long-term memories.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=López-Aranda et alt.|title=Role of Layer 6 of V2 Visual Cortex in Object Recognition Memory|journal=Science|date=2009|volume=325|issue=5936|pages=87–89|doi=10.1126/science.1170869|pmid=19574389}}</ref>
== Third visual cortex, including area V3 ==
The term '''third visual complex''' refers to the region of cortex located immediately in front of V2, which includes the region named '''visual area V3''' in humans. The "complex" nomenclature is justified by the fact that some controversy still exists regarding the exact extent of area V3, with some researchers proposing that the cortex located in front of V2 may include two or three functional subdivisions. For example, David Van Essen and others (1986) have proposed the existence of a "dorsal V3" in the upper part of the cerebral hemisphere, which is distinct from the "ventral V3" (or ventral posterior area, VP) located in the lower part of the brain. Dorsal and ventral V3 have distinct connections with other parts of the brain, appear different in sections stained with a variety of methods, and contain neurons that respond to different combinations of visual stimulus (for example, colour-selective neurons are more common in the ventral V3). Additional subdivisions, including V3A and V3B have also been reported in humans. These subdivisions are located near dorsal V3, but do not adjoin V2.
Dorsal V3 is normally considered to be part of the dorsal stream, receiving inputs from V2 and from the primary visual area and projecting to the posterior [[parietal cortex]]. It may be anatomically located in [[Brodmann area 19]]. Braddick using fMRI has suggested that area V3/V3A may play a role in the processing of [[global motion]]<ref name="Braddick2001">{{cite journal | author=Braddick, OJ, O'Brien, JM| year=2001 | title=Brain areas sensitive to coherent visual motion | journal=Perception | volume=30 | pages=61–72 | doi = 10.1068/p3048 | pmid=11257978 | issue=1|display-authors=etal}}</ref> Other studies prefer to consider dorsal V3 as part of a larger area, named the [[dorsomedial area]] (DM), which contains a representation of the entire visual field. Neurons in area DM respond to [[coherent motion]] of large patterns covering extensive portions of the visual field (Lui and collaborators, 2006).
Ventral V3 (VP), has much weaker connections from the primary visual area, and stronger connections with the [[inferior temporal cortex]]. While earlier studies proposed that VP contained a representation of only the upper part of the visual field (above the point of fixation), more recent work indicates that this area is more extensive than previously appreciated, and like other visual areas it may contain a complete visual representation. The revised, more extensive VP is referred to as the ventrolateral posterior area (VLP) by Rosa and Tweedale.<ref>{{cite journal | doi = 10.1002/1096-9861(20000710)422:4<621::AID-CNE10>3.0.CO;2-E | last1 = Rosa | first1 = MG | last2 = Tweedale | first2 = R |name-list-format=vanc | year = 2000 | title = Visual areas in lateral and ventral extrastriate cortices of the marmoset monkey | url = | journal = J Comp Neurol | volume = 422 | issue = 4| pages = 621–51 | pmid = 10861530 }}</ref>
== V4 ==
{{See also|Color center}}
'''Visual area V4''' is one of the visual areas in the [[extrastriate]] visual cortex. In [[Rhesus macaque|macaques]], it is located anterior to V2 and posterior to [[Inferior temporal gyrus|posterior inferotemporal area (PIT)]]. It comprises at least four regions (left and right V4d, left and right V4v), and some groups report that it contains rostral and caudal subdivisions as well. It is unknown what the human [[homology (biology)|homologue]] of V4 is, and this issue is currently the subject of much scrutiny.<ref name="GoddardEtAl">{{cite journal | author=Goddard E | year=2011 | title=Color responsiveness argues against a dorsal component of human V4 | journal=Journal of Vision | volume=11 | issue=4 | pages=3 | doi = 10.1167/11.4.3 | last2=Goddard | first2=Erin | pmid=21467155 | last3=McDonald | first3=J. S. | last4=Solomon | first4=S. G. | last5=Clifford | first5=C. W. G.|display-authors=etal}}</ref>
V4 is the third cortical area in the [[Two Streams hypothesis#Ventral stream|ventral stream]], receiving strong feedforward input from V2 and sending strong connections to the [[Inferior temporal gyrus|PIT]]. It also receives direct input from V1, especially for central space. In addition, it has weaker connections to V5 and [[Angular gyrus|dorsal prelunate gyrus]] (DP).
V4 is the first area in the [[Two Streams hypothesis#Ventral stream|ventral stream]] to show strong attentional modulation. Most studies indicate that [[selective attention]] can change firing rates in V4 by about 20%. A seminal paper by Moran and Desimone characterizing these effects was the first paper to find attention effects anywhere in the visual cortex.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Moran|first=J|last2= Desimone|first2=R|title=Selective Attention Gates Visual Processing in the Extrastriate Cortex|journal=Science|date=1985|volume=229|issue=4715|pages=782–4|doi=10.1126/science.4023713|pmid=4023713}}</ref>
Like V2, V4 is tuned for orientation, spatial frequency, and color. Unlike V2, V4 is tuned for object features of intermediate complexity, like simple geometric shapes, although no one has developed a full parametric description of the tuning space for V4. Visual area V4 is not tuned for complex objects such as faces, as areas in the [[Inferior temporal gyrus|inferotemporal cortex]] are.
The firing properties of V4 were first described by [[Semir Zeki]] in the late 1970s, who also named the area. Before that, V4 was known by its anatomical description, the [[prelunate gyrus]]. Originally, Zeki argued that the purpose of V4 was to process color information. Work in the early 1980s proved that V4 was as directly involved in form recognition as earlier cortical areas.{{citation needed|date=March 2016}} This research supported the [[Two Streams hypothesis]], first presented by Ungerleider and Mishkin in 1982.
Recent work has shown that V4 exhibits long-term plasticity,<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Schmid|first1=M. C.|last2=Schmiedt|first2=J. T.|last3=Peters|first3=A. J.|last4=Saunders|first4=R. C.|last5=Maier|first5=A.|last6=Leopold|first6=D. A.|title=Motion-Sensitive Responses in Visual Area V4 in the Absence of Primary Visual Cortex|journal=Journal of Neuroscience|date=27 November 2013|volume=33|issue=48|pages=18740–18745|doi=10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3923-13.2013|url=http://www.jneurosci.org/content/jneuro/33/48/18740.full.pdf}}</ref> encodes stimulus salience, is gated by signals coming from the [[frontal eye fields]],<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Moore|first=Tirin|last2=Armstrong|first2=Katherine M.|title=Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex|url=http://www.nature.com/doifinder/10.1038/nature01341|journal=Nature|volume=421|issue=6921|pages=370–373|doi=10.1038/nature01341}}</ref> and shows changes in the spatial profile of its receptive fields with attention.{{citation needed|date=March 2016}}
== Middle temporal visual area (V5) ==<!-- This section is linked from [[V5]] -->
The '''middle temporal visual area''' ('''MT''' or '''V5''') is a region of extrastriate visual cortex. In several species of both [[New World monkey]]s and [[Old World monkey]]s the MT area contains a high concentration of direction-selective neurons.<ref name="BornBradley" /> The MT in primates is thought to play a major role in the [[motion perception|perception of motion]], the integration of local motion signals into global percepts, and the guidance of some [[Eye movement (sensory)|eye movements]].<ref name="BornBradley">{{cite journal |vauthors=Born R, Bradley D | title = Structure and function of visual area MT | journal = Annu Rev Neurosci | volume = 28 | issue = | pages = 157–89 | year = 2005| pmid = 16022593 | doi = 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131052}}</ref>
=== Connections ===
MT is connected to a wide array of cortical and subcortical brain areas. Its input comes from visual cortical areas V1, V2 and dorsal V3 ([[dorsomedial area]]),<ref name="FellemanVanEssen">{{cite journal |vauthors=Felleman D, Van Essen D | title = Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex | journal = [[Cereb Cortex]] | volume = 1 | issue = 1 | pages = 1–47 | year = 1991| pmid = 1822724 | doi = 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a}}</ref><ref name="UngerleiderDesimone">{{cite journal |vauthors=Ungerleider L, Desimone R | title = Cortical connections of visual area MT in the macaque | journal = J Comp Neurol | volume = 248 | issue = 2 | pages = 190–222 | year = 1986 | pmid = 3722458 | doi = 10.1002/cne.902480204}}</ref> the [[koniocellular]] regions of the [[LGN]],<ref name="Sincich">{{cite journal |vauthors=Sincich L, Park K, Wohlgemuth M, Horton J | title = Bypassing V1: a direct geniculate input to area MT | journal = Nat Neurosci | volume = 7 | issue = 10 | pages = 1123–8 | year = 2004 | pmid = 15378066 | doi = 10.1038/nn1318}}</ref> and the [[pulvinar nuclei|inferior pulvinar]].<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Warner CE, Goldshmit Y, Bourne JA | title = Retinal afferents synapse with relay cells targeting the middle temporal area in the pulvinar and lateral geniculate nuclei | journal = Front Neuroanat | volume = 4 | pages = 8 | year = 2010 | pmid = 20179789 | doi = 10.3389/neuro.05.008.2010 | pmc=2826187}}</ref> The pattern of projections to MT changes somewhat between the representations of the foveal and peripheral visual fields, with the latter receiving inputs from areas located in the midline cortex and [[retrosplenial region]].<ref name="PalmerRosa2006">{{cite journal |vauthors=Palmer SM, Rosa MG | year=2006 | title=A distinct anatomical network of cortical areas for analysis of motion in far peripheral vision | journal=Eur J Neurosci | volume=24 |pages=2389–405 | doi=10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05113.x | pmid=17042793 | issue=8}}</ref>
A standard view is that V1 provides the "most important" input to MT.<ref name="BornBradley" /> Nonetheless, several studies have demonstrated that neurons in MT are capable of responding to visual information, often in a direction-selective manner, even after V1 has been destroyed or inactivated.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Rodman | first1 = HR | last2 = Gross | first2 = CG | last3 = Albright | first3 = TD |name-list-format=vanc | year = 1989 | title = Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. I. Effects of striate cortex removal | url = | journal = J Neurosci | volume = 9 | issue = 6| pages = 2033–50 | pmid = 2723765 }}</ref> Moreover, research by [[Semir Zeki]] and collaborators has suggested that certain types of visual information may reach MT before it even reaches V1.
MT sends its major output to areas located in the cortex immediately surrounding it, including areas FST, MST, and V4t (middle temporal crescent). Other projections of MT target the eye movement-related areas of the frontal and parietal lobes (frontal eye field and lateral intraparietal area).
=== Function ===
The first studies of the [[electrophysiological]] properties of neurons in MT showed that a large portion of the cells are [[neuronal tuning|tuned]] to the speed and direction of moving visual stimuli.<ref name="DubnerZeki">{{cite journal |vauthors=Dubner R, Zeki S | title = Response properties and receptive fields of cells in an anatomically defined region of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey | journal = Brain Res | volume = 35 | issue = 2 | pages = 528–32 | year = 1971 | pmid = 5002708 | doi = 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90494-X}}.</ref><ref name="MaunsellVanEssen">{{cite journal |vauthors=Maunsell J, Van Essen D | title = Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation | journal = J Neurophysiol | volume = 49 | issue = 5 | pages = 1127–47 | year = 1983 | pmid = 6864242}}</ref>
[[Lesion]] studies have also supported the role of MT in motion perception and eye movements.<ref name=Dursteler1987>{{cite journal | title=Directional pursuit deficits following lesions of the foveal representation within the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey |author1=Dursteler M.R. |author2=Wurtz R.H. |author3=Newsome W.T. | journal=Journal of Neurophysiology | year=1987 | volume=57 | issue=5 | pages=1262–87 | pmid=3585468}}</ref> [[Neuropsychology|Neuropsychological]] studies of a patient unable to see motion, seeing the world in a series of static 'frames' instead, suggested that V5 in the primate is homologous to MT in the human.<ref name=Hess1989>{{cite journal | title=The 'motion-blind' patient: low-level spatial and temporal filters |author1=Hess R.H. |author2=Baker C.L. |author3=Zihl J. | journal=Journal of Neuroscience | year=1989 | volume=9 | issue=5 | pages=1628–40 | pmid=2723744}}</ref><ref name=Baker1991>{{cite journal | title=Residual motion perception in a 'motion-blind' patient, assessed with limited-lifetime random dot stimuli |author1=Baker C.L. Jr |author2=Hess R.F |author3=Zihl J. | journal=Journal of Neuroscience | year=1991 | volume=11 | issue=2 | pages=454–61 | pmid=1992012}}</ref>
However, since neurons in V1 are also tuned to the direction and speed of motion, these early results left open the question of precisely what MT could do that V1 could not. Much work has been carried out on this region, as it appears to integrate local visual motion signals into the global motion of complex objects.<ref name="Movshon">Movshon, J.A., Adelson, E.H., Gizzi, M.S., & Newsome, W.T. (1985). The analysis of moving visual patterns. In: C. Chagas, R. Gattass, & C. Gross (Eds.), Pattern recognition mechanisms (pp. 117-151), Rome: Vatican Press.</ref>
For example, ''lesion'' to the V5 leads to deficits in perceiving motion and processing of complex stimuli. It contains many neurons selective for the motion of complex visual features (line ends, corners). ''Microstimulation'' of a neuron located in the V5 affects the perception of motion. For example, if one finds a neuron with preference for upward motion in a monkey's V5 and stimulates it with an electrode, then the monkey becomes more likely to report 'upward' motion when presented with stimuli containing 'left' and 'right' as well as 'upward' components.<ref name="BrittenVanWezel">{{cite journal | title=Electrical microstimulation of cortical area MST biases heading perception in monkeys |author1=Britten K.H. |author2=van Wezel R.J. | journal=Nat. Neurosci. | year=1998 | volume=1 | issue=1 | pages=59–63 | pmid=10195110 | doi=10.1038/259}}</ref>
There is still much controversy over the exact form of the computations carried out in area MT<ref name="Wilson">{{cite journal | last1 = Wilson | first1 = H.R. | last2 = Ferrera | first2 = V.P. | last3 = Yo | first3 = C. | year = 1992 | title = A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception | url = | journal = Vis Neurosci | volume = 9 | issue = 1| pages = 79–97 | doi=10.1017/s0952523800006386}}</ref> and some research suggests that feature motion is in fact already available at lower levels of the visual system such as V1.
<ref name="Tinsley">{{cite journal | title=The nature of V1 neural responses to 2D moving patterns depends on receptive-field structure in the marmoset monkey | author=Tinsley, C.J., Webb, B.S., Barraclough, N.E., Vincent, C.J., Parker, A., & Derrington, A.M.| journal=J Neurophysiol | year=2003 | volume=90 | issue=2 | pages=930–7 | pmid=12711710 | doi=10.1152/jn.00708.2002}}</ref>
<ref name="PackBorn">{{cite journal | title=Two-dimensional substructure of stereo and motion interactions in macaque visual cortex |author1=Pack C.C. |author2=Born R.T. |author3=Livingstone M.S. | journal=Neuron | year=2003 | volume=37 | issue=3 | pages=525–35 | pmid=12575958 | doi=10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01187-x}}</ref>
=== Functional organization ===
MT was shown to be organized in direction columns.<ref name="Albright">{{cite journal | author = Albright T | title = Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque | journal = J Neurophysiol | volume = 52 | issue = 6 | pages = 1106–30 | year = 1984 | pmid = 6520628}}</ref> DeAngelis argued that MT neurons were also organized based on their tuning for binocular disparity.<ref name="DeAngelisNewsome">{{cite journal |vauthors=DeAngelis G, Newsome W | title = Organization of disparity-selective neurons in macaque area MT | journal = J Neurosci | volume = 19 | issue = 4 | pages = 1398–415 | year = 1999 | pmid = 9952417}}</ref>
==V6==
<!-- [[Dorsomedial area]] redirects to this section -->
The '''dorsomedial area''' (DM) also known as '''V6''', appears to respond to visual stimuli associated with self-motion <ref>{{cite journal|last1=Cardin|first1=V|last2=Smith|first2=AT|title=Sensitivity of human visual and vestibular cortical regions to stereoscopic depth gradients associated with self-motion|date=2010|volume=20|issue=8|pages=1964–73|doi=10.1093/cercor/bhp268|journal=Cerebral Cortex}}</ref> and wide-field stimulation.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Pitzalis et alt.|title=Wide-Field Retinotopy Defines Human Cortical Visual Area V6|journal=The Journal of Neuroscience|date=2006|volume=26|issue=30|pages=7962–73|pmid=16870741|doi=10.1523/jneurosci.0178-06.2006}}</ref> V6, is a subdivision of the visual cortex of primates first described by [[John Allman]] and [[Jon Kaas]] in 1975.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Allman JM, Kaas JH | year = 1975 | title = The dorsomedial cortical visual area: a third tier area in the occipital lobe of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus) | url = | journal = Brain Res | volume = 100 | issue = 3| pages = 473–487 | doi=10.1016/0006-8993(75)90153-5}}</ref> V6 is located in the dorsal part of the [[extrastriate cortex]], near the deep groove through the centre of the brain ([[medial longitudinal fissure]]), and typically also includes portions of the medial cortex, such as the [[parieto-occipital sulcus]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} DM contains a topographically organized representation of the entire field of vision.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
There are similarities between the visual area V5 and V6 of the [[common marmoset]]. Both areas receive direct connections from the [[primary visual cortex]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} And both have a high [[myelin]] content, a characteristic that is usually present in brain structures involved in fast transmission of information.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
For many years, it was considered that DM only existed in [[New World monkeys]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} However, more recent research has suggested that DM also exists in [[Old World monkeys]] and perhaps humans.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} V6 is also sometimes referred to as the parieto-occipital area (PO), although the correspondence is not exact.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Galletti C, etal | year = 2005 | title = The relationship between V6 and PO in macaque extrastriate cortex | url = http://www.gallettilab.unibo.it/Pdf/Galletti et al 2005.pdf| journal = Eur J Neurosci | volume = 21 | issue = | pages = 959–970 | doi=10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03911.x}}</ref><ref name="Gal">{{cite journal |vauthors=Galletti C, etal | year = 2003 | title = Role of the medial parieto-occipital cortex in the control of reaching and grasping movements | url = | journal = Exp Brain Res | volume = 153 | issue = | pages = 158–170 | doi=10.1007/s00221-003-1589-z}}</ref>
===Properties===
Neurons in area DM/V6 of [[night monkey]]s and [[common marmoset]]s have unique response properties, including an extremely sharp selectivity for the orientation of visual contours, and preference for long, uninterrupted lines covering large parts of the visual field.<ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Baker JF, etal | year = 1981 | title = Visual response properties of neurons in four extrastriate visual areas of the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus): a quantitative comparison of medial, dorsomedial, dorsolateral, and middle temporal areas | url = | journal = J Neurophysiol | volume = 45 | issue = | pages = 397–416 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |vauthors=Lui LL, etal | year = 2006 | title = Functional response properties of neurons in the dorsomedial visual area of New World monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) | url = | journal = Cereb Cortex | volume = 16 | issue = 2| pages = 162–177 | doi=10.1093/cercor/bhi094}}</ref>
However, in comparison with area MT, a much smaller proportion of DM cells shows selectivity for the direction of motion of visual patterns.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} Another notable difference with area MT is that cells in DM are attuned to low spatial frequency components of an image, and respond poorly to the motion of textured patterns such as a field of random dots.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} These response properties suggest that DM and MT may work in parallel, with the former analyzing self-motion relative to the environment, and the latter analyzing the motion of individual objects relative to the background.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
Recently, an area responsive to wide-angle flow fields has been identified in the human and is thought to be a homologue of macaque area V6.<ref>{{cite journal | authors=Pitzalis, S., Sereno, M.I., Committeri, G., Fattori, P., Galati, G., Patria, F., & Galletti, C. | year=2010 | title=Human v6: The medial motion area. | journal=Cereb Cortex | volume=20 | issue=2 | pages=411–424 | doi=10.1093/cercor/bhp112}}</ref>
===Pathways===
The connections and response properties of cells in DM/ V6 suggest that this area is a key node in a subset of the '[[dorsal stream]]', referred to by some as the 'dorsomedial pathway'.{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} This pathway is likely to be important for the control of skeletomotor activity, including postural reactions and reaching movements towards objects <ref name="Gal"/> The main 'feedforward' connection of DM is to the cortex immediately rostral to it, in the interface between the occipital and parietal lobes (V6A).{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}} This region has, in turn, relatively direct connections with the regions of the frontal lobe that control arm movements, including the [[premotor cortex]].{{Citation needed|date=November 2016}}
== See also ==
{{Portal|Neuroscience}}
* [[Cortical area]]
* [[Cortical blindness]]
* [[Feature integration theory]]
* [[List of regions in the human brain]]
* [[Retinotopy]]
* [[Visual processing]]
* [[Visual feature array]]
* [[Complex cell]]
==References==
{{Reflist|30em}}
==External links==
{{Commons category|Visual cortex}}
* [http://webvision.med.utah.edu/VisualCortex.html The Primary Visual Cortex by Matthew Schmolesky] at [[University of Utah]]
* [http://hubel.med.harvard.edu/book/b23.htm Architecture of the Visual Cortex, by David Hubel] at [[Harvard University]]
* {{BrainInfo|ancil|415}} - striate area 17
* {{BrainInfo|ancil|699}} - Brodmann area 17 in guenon
* {{BrainMaps|visual%20cortex}}
* [http://topographica.org Simulator for computational modeling of visual cortex maps] at topographica.org
{{Prosencephalon}}
{{Visual system}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Visual Cortex}}
[[Category:Visual cortex]]
[[Category:Visual perception]]' |