Jump to content

Role of serotonin in visual orientation processing

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Kiri Glintborg (talk | contribs) at 04:46, 6 June 2016 (Created page with '<!-- Don't mess with this line! -->{{subst:unreviewed}} <!-- Write your article below this line --> ==The Role of Serotonin in Visual Orientation Processing==...'). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.
(diff) ← Previous revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)

Template:New unreviewed article

The Role of Serotonin in Visual Orientation Processing

Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine) is a monoamine neurotransmitter that plays a role in mood, eating, sleeping, arousal and potentially visual orientation processing [1] [[1]] [2] [3] [4] [5]. To investigate its function in visual orientation, researchers have utilised MDMA, or as it is commonly referred to, Ecstasy (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) [[2]] [6] [7] [8]. MDMA is known to affect serotonin neurons in the brain and cause neurotoxicity [6] [9] [7] [10] [11] [12]. Serotonin has been hypothesised to be involved in visual orientation because individuals who use MDMA exhibit an increase in the magnitude of the tilt aftereffect (TAE) [3] [6] [7] [8][13] [14]. The TAE is a visual illusion where viewing lines in one direction, for an extended period of time, produces the perception of a tilt in the opposite direction to vertical lines subsequently viewed [[4]] [6] [7] [15] [16]. This effect is proposed to occur due to lateral inhibition to orientation sensitive neurons in the occipital lobe [17] [8]. Lateral inhibition is where neurons that become activated to a particular orientation send inhibitory signals to their neighbouring neurons [[5]] [16] [18]. The degree of orientation that each neuron becomes maximally excited to is referred to as the ‘tuning bandwidth’ [[6]] [19] [7] [16]. Lateral inhibition consequently plays a pivotal role in each neuron’s tuning bandwidth, such that if lateral inhibition no longer occurs, a greater number of neurons will become stimulated to the same orientation [7]. This results in the activated neurons becoming adapted to the same orientation stimulus, if the stimulus is viewed for a period of time. As a consequence, if those neurons are subsequently ‘shown’ another stimulus that differs slightly in its orientation, those neurons are no longer able to achieve the same level of response as compared to other non-adapted neurons [16].

Image Displaying the Serotonin Nerve Pathways in the Brain


Example of the Tilt Aftereffect

Studies have consequently utilised the TAE to assess the degree of lateral inhibition that occurs from MDMA use. The results of these studies have found evidence to support the role that serotonin plays in visual orientation [6] [7] [8][16]. This was evidenced through individuals who solely used MDMA reporting a greater magnitude of the TAE compared to drug naive controls [6] [7]. This increased magnitude showed that serotonin plays a role in lateral inhibition by potentially having a honing effect, meaning that orientation neurons become maximally excited to their preferred orientation, and less so to others [6]. This additionally provides further evidence of the neurotoxicity of MDMA [6] [8]. This area of research, overall, has provided insights into the mechanisms of visual orientation processing and the effect that MDMA neurotoxicity has on this system. This furthers the understanding of both the role that serotonin has on the visual system and to what degree MDMA neurotoxicity affects the brain.

History and Effect of MDMA

Ecstasy is the street name that refers to the popular recreational drug that contains 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) [20]. The now frequently used drug in the rave and club scene was first synthesized by Merck, a German pharmaceutical company that was investigating the development of new medications in the early 1900s [21]. Since its development, it has undergone various phases, from controversially being used as a therapeutic aid in the 1970s, to being banned in the 1980s after the Drug Enforcement Administration concluded that it was addictive [22] [23] [24]. During and following the 1970s, however, MDMA became a popular recreational drug due to it producing feelings of euphoria, empathy, social closeness, mild hallucinations and stimulation [6] Cite error: A <ref> tag is missing the closing </ref> (see the help page). [25].


MDMA is part of the amphetamine family and elicits its positive effects by altering brain serotonin, dopamine and norepinephrine neurotransmitter levels [22] [23] [24] [6]. As the drug begins to take effect, the brain becomes flooded with serotonin which can then become depleted within 3-6 hours following consumption [22]. It has also been shown that an enzyme required to synthesize serotonin becomes deactivated, therefore, inhibiting the brain’s replenishment of used serotonin [22]. Due to changes that the brain undergoes during and following MDMA consumption, various consequences have been noted. These have included memory impairment, anxiety, paranoia, mood swings and depression [22] [23] . This has raised further concerns as to what extent MDMA may damage and change the brain’s chemistry and what this means for its users Cite error: A <ref> tag is missing the closing </ref> (see the help page).. This reduction in serotonin resulted in an increase in the magnitude of the TAE in those subjects [26]. This study has since been used as the foundation for the idea that MDMA neurotoxicity, due to its effect on serotonin neurons, could influence the magnitude of the TAE in individuals who use MDMA [6] [7] [8].

Present Research

Current findings regarding altered visual orientation processing from MDMA use comes from research by White, Brown and Edwards (2013) [6]. Their study sought to extend the results found in previous research, such as Maisini et al. (1990),[27] and investigate how MDMA affects visual processing in the occipital lobe. The participants of the study were divided into three groups: Ecstasy users who were amphetamine abstinent, Ecstasy users who also used amphetamines, and drug naive control participants [6]. Ecstasy users who additionally used amphetamine were included as results from prior studies have indicated that concurrent amphetamine use may mediate the effects of MDMA on orientation neurons [19] [7] [8].

The results of the study indicated that the amphetamine abstinent Ecstasy group showed a broader tuning bandwidth than the controls [6]. This demonstrates that MDMA use produces changes to serotonergic functioning as it disrupts lateral inhibition between orientation sensitive neurons. This disruption causes the neurons to activate to a wider range of orientations other than their preferred orientation [6] [7]. This finding, therefore, supports the idea that serotonin plays a role in sharpening the tuning bandwidths of orientation neurons [6]. Overall, the results support the idea that “MDMA-mediated serotonin depletion can lead to broader orientation tuning bandwidths” [6] p.163. The authors do, however, go on to say that although deficits in certain tasks are present, the extent of these deficits requires further investigation [6].

A study by Brown, Edwards, McKone and Ward (2007), [7] additionally investigated MDMA's effect on serotonin neurons. Their research also stemmed from Masini et al. (1990) [28]. They were interested in serotonin's role in lateral inhibition to orientation sensitive neurons and how MDMA use may change this system and produce wider tuning bandwidths [29]. The study consisted of two groups, Ecstasy users and controls, who were shown brief displays of the TAE illusion [7]. The results of the study support the idea that serotonin damage due to MDMA use causes lateral inhibition to diminish amongst orientation sensitive neurons in the occipital lobe [7]. This was demonstrated by the Ecstasy group showing a greater increase in the magnitude of the TAE illusion compared to the controls [7]. The authors stated that perhaps “serotonin is involved in the extent to which the sensitivity of neurons is reduced during adaptation” [7] p.445. It could be that the decrease in sensitivity of the post-adaptation orientation neurons is further diminished by decreased serotonergic functioning, which increases the magnitude of the TAE. Their research lends support to the idea that MDMA use affects lateral inhibition and that serotonin plays a role in visual orientation processing [7].


References

  1. ^ Carlson, Neil (2014). Physiology of Behaviour (Eleventh ed.). England: Pearson Education Limited. pp. 121–122. ISBN 10: 1-292-02320-1. {{cite book}}: Check |isbn= value: invalid character (help)
  2. ^ White, Clare; John, Brown; Mark, Edwards (2013). "Altered visual perception in long-term ecstasy (MDMA) users". Psychopharmacology. 229. doi:DOI 10.1007/s00213-013-3094-9. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  3. ^ Brown, John; Mark, Edwards; Elinor, Mckone; Jeff, Ward (2007). "A long-term ecstasy-related change in visual perception". Psychopharmacology. 193 (3): 437–446. doi:DOI 10.1007/s00213-007-0785-0. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  4. ^ Dickson, C; Bruno, R; Brown, J (2009). "Investigating the Role of Serotonin in Visual Orientation Processing Using an 'Ecstasy' (MDMA)-Based Research Model". Neuropsychobiology. 60: 204–212. doi:DOI:10.1159/000253556. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  5. ^ Murry, Elizabeth; Bruno, Raimondo; Brown, John (2012). "Residual effects of ecstasy (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) on low level visual processes". Human Psychopharmacology. 27 (2): 226–234. doi:10.1002/hup.2218.
  6. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r Cite error: The named reference White was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  7. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Cite error: The named reference Brown was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  8. ^ a b c d e f g Cite error: The named reference Dickson was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  9. ^ Cite error: The named reference Carlson was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  10. ^ Ricaurte, George; McCann, Una (2001). "Experimental studies on 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, "ECSTASY") and its potential to damage brain serotonin neurons". Neurotoxicity Research. 3 (1): 85–99. doi:10.1007/BF03033232.
  11. ^ Morley, Kirsten; Li, Kong; Hunt, Glenn; Mallet, Paul; McGregor, Ian (2004). "Cannabinoids prevent the acute hyperthermia and partially protect against the 5-HT depleting effects of MDMA ("Ecstasy") in rats". Neuropharmacology. 46 (7): 954–965. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.01.002.
  12. ^ McCann, U.D.; Szabo, Z; Scheffel, U.; Dannals, R.F.; Ricaurte, G.A. (1998). "Positron emission tomographic evidence of toxic effect of MDMA ("ecstasy") on brain serotonin neurons in human beings". ProQuest Health & Medical Complete. 352: 1433–37.
  13. ^ Fisk, John; Catharine, Montgomery; Florentina, Hadjiefthyvoulou (2011). "Visuospatial working memory impairment in current and previous ecstasy/polydrug users". Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical & Experimental. 26 (4/5): 313–321. doi:10.1002/hup.1207.
  14. ^ Murry, Elizabeth; Bruno, Raimondo; Brown, John (2012). "Residual effects of ecstasy (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) on low level visual processes". Human Psychopharmacology. 27 (226–234). doi:10.1002/hup.2218.
  15. ^ Wenderoth, Peter; smith, Stuart (1999). "Neural substrates of the tilt illusion". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Ophthalmology. 27 (3–4): 271-274. doi:DOI: 10.1046/j.1440-1606.1999.00191.x. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  16. ^ a b c d e Cite error: The named reference Murry was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  17. ^ Cite error: The named reference Wenderoth was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  18. ^ Vaitkevicius, Henrikas; Villiunas, Villius; Bliumas, Remigijus; Stanikunas, Rytis; Svegzda, Algimontas; Dzekeviciute, Aldona; Kulikowski, Janos (2009). "Influences of prolonged viewing of tilted lines on perceived line orientation: the normalization and tilt after-effect". Journal of the Optical Society of America A. 26 (7): 1553–1563. doi:10.1364/JOSAA.26.001553.
  19. ^ a b Cite error: The named reference white was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  20. ^ Rogers, G; Elston, J; Garside, R; Roome, C; Taylor, R; Younger, P; Zawada, A; Somerville, M (2009). "Harmful health effects of recreational ecstasy: Systematic Review of Observational Evidence". Health Technology Assessment. 13 (6). doi:10.3310/hta13060.
  21. ^ Harris, Gardenia (2008). "What All Social Workers Should Know About MDMA (Ecstasy)". Journal of Social Work in the Addictions. 4 (1): 23–37. doi:10.1300/J160v04n01 03. {{cite journal}}: Check |doi= value (help)
  22. ^ a b c d e Cite error: The named reference Harris was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  23. ^ a b c Cite error: The named reference Rogers was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  24. ^ a b Cite error: The named reference Ricaurte was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  25. ^ Fischer, C; Hatzidimitrou, G; Wlos, J; Katz, J; Ricaurte, G (1995). "Reorganization of ascending 5ht axon projections in animals previously exposed to the recreational drug MDMA". Journal of Neuroscience. 15 (8): 5478–5485.
  26. ^ Masini, Roberto; Antionetti, Alessandro; Moja, Egidio (1990). "An Increase in the Strength of Tilt aftereffect Associated with Tryptophan Depletion". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 70: 531–539. doi:10.2466/pms.1990.70.2.531.
  27. ^ Cite error: The named reference Masini was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  28. ^ Cite error: The named reference Masini' was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  29. ^ Cite error: The named reference Brown' was invoked but never defined (see the help page).