Jump to content

Extended parallel process model

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Teenytinyhooman (talk | contribs) at 02:41, 11 February 2022 (Changed the sentence structure and word usage so it is understandable for lay-persons and the general Wikipedia audience.). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

The extended parallel process model (EPPM) is a fear appeal theory developed by communications scholar Kim Witte that evaluates how individuals react to fear-inducing messages. It was first published in Communication Monographs, Volume 59, December 1992; Witte subsequently published an initial test of the model in a later article in Communication Monographs, Volume 61, June 1994.

EPPM was developed by Witte as a response to the significant inconsistencies in fear appeal literature, serving as an extension of previous fear appeal models, hence the 'extended' in EPPM. The model is based initially on Leventhal's Parallel Process Model - a danger and fear control framework that studied how adaptive protective behaviour stemmed from attempts of danger control.[1] It also significantly draws from Roger's Protection motivation theory, which proposes two responses to fear-inducing stimuli: threat appraisal and coping appraisal.[2]

The model's main theory is that when confronted with a fear-inducing stimulus, humans tend to engage in two simultaneous ways of message processing: a perceived efficacy appraisal (cognitive processing) and a perceived threat appraisal (emotional processing). Differences in message appraisal then lead to two behavioural outcomes, with individuals engaging in either a danger control process or a fear control process. In the case of the message being perceived as having no element of threat, individuals do not exhibit a response, and the message is ignored.

Witte's EPPM expands on previous fear appeal studies by explaining the reasons for failure in fear appeals and reincorporating fear as a central variable in the model. The first fear appeal model also outlines the relationship between threat and efficacy in propositional forms.

The EPPM concludes that a fear control process leads to message rejection, while a danger control process leads to message acceptance, leading to adaptive behavioural changes.

Background

Two main components of large-scale public messaging that induce behavioural change are fear appeals and fear appraisals. Fear appeals are specifically designed to elicit fear and nudge individuals to adapt to the recommendations outlined in the message and find their use in public health campaigns and political adverts. Appeals are designed to fit three main categories: message, behaviour, and the audience.[3]

  • Message: The content that is included in the fear-inducing message
  • Behaviour: The behavioural response recommended by the message
  • Audience: The characteristics of the audience receiving the message

Fear appraisals are the mental evaluations made in response to experiencing fear-inducing stimuli and are also known as threat appraisals. Fear appeal literature is primarily focused on understanding and deriving key fear appraisal processes in humans, with the intention of using it to drive social campaigns and behavioural interventions.[4]

Witte's motivations for designing an updated fear appeal model was, as described by her, due to the declining role of fear in fear appeals. While initially, fear was the pinnacle of theoretical fear appeal literature, it was starting to be considered as a control variable in subsequent models. A lack of precision in the Parallel Process Model and empirical inconsistencies in the Protection Motivation Theory were also noted by Witte as reasons for formulating an extended parallel process model.

The EPPM, in addition to message acceptance processes outlined in the Parallel Process Model, explores and expands on the fear control processes that cause message rejection.

EPPM Components

The EPPM uses persuasive fear-inducing messages to induce intended behavioural responses. Wittle details three main processes involved in fear appraisal: the fear appeal inputs, the message processing of the inputs, and the outputs, or action taken after evaluating the perceived threat.

Fear Appeal Inputs

Illustration of the Extended Parallel Process Model.

According to fear appeal studies, a fear appeal has two components: a component of threat and a component of efficacy. These two components are further divided into two categories each. Severity and susceptibility make up the threat component, and response-efficacy and self-efficacy make up the efficacy component. These four key factors, as defined by the EPPM, predict the likely outcome of communications that involve a fear appeal:

Threat Variables

  • Susceptibility – The perception the individual has of how likely the threat is to impact them.
  • Severity – The perception the individual has of the magnitude of the threat.

Efficacy Variables

  • Self-Efficacy – The perception the individual has that they are competent to perform the tasks needed to control the risk.
  • Response Efficacy – The perception the individual has that the action, if carried out, will successfully control the risk.

The outcome of fear appeals is determined by an appraisal, that is, the evaluation of the message as either dangerous or indifferent.

Fear Appraisal

Appraisal Theory states that an individual makes either an emotional or affective response to external stimuli. The EPPM outlines two primary appraisals an individual makes in response to a fear appeal: a threat appraisal, followed by an efficacy appraisal.

  • Low threat appraisal: when a threat appraisal is perceived to be low, i.e., there is a lack of imminent threat, the fear appeal is rejected immediately.
  • Moderate to High threat appraisal: When a threat appraisal is perceived as moderate or high, fear is induced, and individuals begin the efficacy appraisal.
  • Low efficacy appraisal: when the efficacy appraisal is perceived to be low, the message does not induce behavioural change.
  • Moderate to High efficacy appraisal: when the efficacy appraisal is perceived as moderate or high, the message induces a behavioural change.

After appraisals of the fear appeal, individuals then take action based on whether the threat is imminent or trivial.

Fear Appeal Outcomes

The EPPM predicts three possible outcomes after the fear appraisal is carried out:

Danger control – When an individual perceives that the severity and susceptibility are high (i.e., high threat appraisal) and also perceives that they are competent to take mitigating action (i.e., high efficacy appraisal), then they are likely to act to control the danger.

Fear control– The model predicts that if an individual perceives their ability to control risk as low, even if the severity and susceptibility are perceived as high, they are likely to take steps to control their fear instead. Fear control responses are defined as coping mechanisms that reduce fear and include denial, psychological reactance and defensive avoidance. These are maladaptive changes, or counterproductive behaviours. Fear controlling behaviour may involve the use of cognitive defence mechanisms such as "It will happen to me sooner or later" in order to manage the state of anxiety.

No Response – The severity or susceptibility of the danger is perceived as low, and the individual rejects the message. There is no behavioural change.

See also

Original articles

  • Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 59(4), 329–349.
  • Witte, K. (1994). Fear control and danger control: A test of the extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 61(2), 113–134.

References

  1. ^ Leventhal, H (1971-06). "Fear appeals and persuasion: the differentiation of a motivational construct". American Journal of Public Health. 61 (6): 1208–1224. doi:10.2105/AJPH.61.6.1208. ISSN 0090-0036. PMC 1529874. PMID 4110702. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  2. ^ Rogers, Ronald W. (1975-09). "A Protection Motivation Theory of Fear Appeals and Attitude Change1". The Journal of Psychology. 91 (1): 93–114. doi:10.1080/00223980.1975.9915803. ISSN 0022-3980. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  3. ^ Tannenbaum, Melanie B.; Hepler, Justin; Zimmerman, Rick S.; Saul, Lindsey; Jacobs, Samantha; Wilson, Kristina; Albarracín, Dolores (November 2015). "Appealing to fear: A meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories". Psychological Bulletin. 141 (6): 1178–1204. doi:10.1037/a0039729. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 5789790. PMID 26501228.
  4. ^ Maloney, E.; Popova, L (2011). "The Extended Parallel Process Model: Illuminating the Gaps in Research". Health Education and Behavior. 39: 455–473. doi:10.1177/1090198111418108.