Jump to content

Syntax–semantics interface

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Botterweg (talk | contribs) at 18:10, 11 August 2021 (trying to characterize relevant topics more broadly (happy to discuss if there's any disagreement)). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

In linguistics, the syntax–semantics interface is the interaction between syntax and semantics. Its study encompasses phenomena that pertain to both syntax and semantics, with the goal of explaining correlations between form and meaning.[1] Specific topics include scope,[2][3] binding,[2] and lexical semantic properties such as verbal aspect and nominal individuation.[4][5][6][7][8]

The interface is conceived of very differently in formalist and functionalist approaches. While functionalists tend to look into semantics and pragmatics for explanations of syntactic phenomena, formalists try to limit such explanations within syntax itself.[9] It is sometimes referred to as the morphosyntax–semantics interface or the syntax-lexical semantics interface.[3]

Functionalist approaches

Within functionalist approaches, research on the syntax–semantics interface has been aimed at disproving the formalist argument of the autonomy of syntax, by finding instances of semantically determined syntactic structures.[4]

Van Valin and LaPolla, in their 1997 monographic study, found that the more semantically motivated or driven a syntactic phenomena is, the more it tends to be topologically universal, that is, to show less cross-linguistic variation.[10]

Formal approaches

Within the paradigm of formal semantics, the syntax–semantics interface is viewed as a mechanism for semantically interpreting syntactic structures. In the Heim and Kratzer model commonly adopted within generative linguistics, denotations are read off a level of syntactic representation called logical form. At logical form, semantic relationships such as scope and binding are represented unambiguously, having been determined by operations such as quantifier raising. Thus, this model places much of the explanatory burden in the syntactic rather than the semantic component of the grammar. Other formal frameworks take the opposite approach, using mechanisms such as type shifting and dynamic binding to compute denotations based on surface structures.[1][11][12][2]

Levin and Rappaport Hovav, in their 1995 monograph, reiterated that there are some aspects of verb meaning that are relevant to syntax, and others that are not, as previously noted by Steven Pinker.[13][14] Levin and Rappaport Hovav isolated such aspects focusing on the phenomenon of unaccusativity that is "semantically determined and syntactivally encoded".[15]

History

Before the 1950s, there was no discussion of a syntax–semantics interface in American linguistics, since neither semantics nor semantics was an active area of research.[16] This neglect was due in part to the influence of logical positivism and behaviorism in psychology, that viewed hypotheses about linguistic meaning as untestable.[16][17]

By the 1960s, syntax had become a major area of study, and some researchers began examining semantics as well. In this period, the most prominent view of the interface was the Katz-Postal Hypothesis according to which deep structure was the level of syntactic representation which underwent semantic interpretation. This assumption was upended by data involving quantifiers, which showed that syntactic transformations can affect meaning. During the linguistics wars, a variety of competing notions of the interface were developed, many of which live on in present day work.[16][2]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ a b Chierchia (1999)
  2. ^ a b c d Partee (2014)
  3. ^ a b Hackl (2013)
  4. ^ a b Cite error: The named reference Levin1995 was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  5. ^ Van Valin & LaPolla (1997)
  6. ^ Vendler (1957)
  7. ^ Tenny (1994)
  8. ^ Cite error: The named reference VanValin2005p67 was invoked but never defined (see the help page).
  9. ^ Van Valin 2003, p.334
  10. ^ Van Valin (2005), ch.5 "Linking syntactic and semantic representations in simple sentences" p.128
  11. ^ Heim & Kratzer (1998)
  12. ^ Baker (2015)
  13. ^ Levin & Rappaport Hovav (1995) ch.1 p. 9
  14. ^ Pinker 1989
  15. ^ Levin & Rappaport Hovav (1995) ch.5 p.179, Afterword p.279
  16. ^ a b c Partee (2014).pp.2, 6
  17. ^ Taylor (2017)

References

  • Barker, Chris (2015). "Scope" (PDF). In Lappin, Shalom; Fox, Chris (eds.). Handbook of Contemporary Semantics (2 ed.). Wiley Blackwell. Section 4.3. doi:10.1002/9781118882139.ch2.
  • Chierchia, G. (1999) Syntax-semantics interface, pp. 824-826, in: The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences, Edited by Keil & Wilson (1999) Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Hackl, M. (2013) The syntax–semantics interface. Lingua, 130, 66-87.
  • Heim, Irene; Kratzer, Angelika (1998). Semantics in Generative Grammar. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell. pp. 194–198.
  • Levin, B., & Pinker, S. (1992) Introduction in Beth Levin & Steven Pinker (1992, Eds) Lexical & conceptual semantics. (A Cognition Special Issue) Cambridge, MA and Oxford: Blackwell, 1991. Pp. 244.
  • Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1995). Unaccusativity: At the syntax–lexical semantics interface. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  • Partee, Barbara (2014). "A brief history of the syntax-semantics interface in Western formal linguistics" (PDF). Semantics-Syntax Interface. 1 (1): 1–20. [1]
  • Pinker, S. (1989) Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. New editoin in 2013: Learnability and Cognition, new edition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure. MIT press.
  • Taylor, J. (2017) Lexical Semantics. In B. Dancygier (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (Cambridge Handbooks in Language and Linguistics, pp. 246-261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/9781316339732.017
  • Tenny, C. (1994). Aspectual roles and the syntax-semantics interface (Vol. 52). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
  • Van Valin, R. D. Jr. & LaPolla, R. J. (1997) Syntax: Structure, meaning, and function. Cambridge University Press.
  • Van Valin Jr, R. D. (2003) Functional linguistics, ch. 13 in The handbook of linguistics, pp. 319-336.
  • Van Valin, R. D. Jr. (2005). Exploring the syntax-semantics interface, Cambridge University Press.
  • Vendler, Z. (1957) Verbs and times in The Philosophical Review 66(2): 143–160. Reprinted as ch. 4 of Linguistics and Philosophy, Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press 1967, pp.97-121.

Further reading